General
remarks :Language
performance is a highly complex activity. The tongue and lips must make skilled
movements and there must be highly coordinated muscular activity involving even
areas which are not directly involved in the production of speech. The speech
is performed so rapidly that we are not even aware of the process. But speech
is more than just a complex motor activity. It is also a complex mental activity
which involves the acquisition of the language structures, associating words,
objects and concepts and so on. The acquisition, storage and retrieval of language
involve the Central Nervous System.
We will present here a sketch essay on the known structure of the Central Nervous
System (CNS) and its role in the production and control of speech. We will present
also certain important neuropsychological theories proposed as constructs of the
process that underlie human behaviour. Then we will identify and describe pathologies
of speech from the neurophysiological and linguistic background.
Neurophysiology
: network of nerves :
Our knowledge about the structure and functioning of the human nervous system
and the brain continues to be scanty in spite of intense research activity in
this area. We know now a great deal about the anatomy of the nervous system. We
know also about the neuron which is the basis of the nervous system. But nothing
in detail is known about how any one particular function is carried on in the
nervous system; nor do we have answers to questions such as what happens in our
brain when we are involved in language activity, how do we associate a word with
an idea, and what is its neurological correspondence, how do we associate a word
with an idea acquired long ago, what is the corresponding neural activity for
the thought which occurs before we start speaking, what is the precise nature
of the storage and retrieval system that must underlie language and all human
behaviour.
The nervous system consists of billions of neurons which are interconnected. These
neurons are the basis of the nervous system. The neuron is a living nerve cell.
It is a small mass of protoplasm covered by a semi-permeable membrane. This membrane
allows the materials essential for maintaining the cell's life to enter and the
waste products to go out. The neurons have some features common to all of them.
These include cell body, cell nucleus, the axon or nerve fibre, terminal arbor,
dendrites and synapses. (See Fig-1).
Fig.
1. A typical neuron
The cell body contains the cell nucleus. The axon is a fine filament which extends
from the cell body and which can be even several feet long, as in the case of
the axon from the spinal column to the muscles which control the movement of the
toes. The axon has several side branches and these side branches terminate as
the terminal arbor in a fine network of filaments. Dendrites are extensions that
sprout from the cell body of a different neuron. Synapses are the junctions at
which connections between neurons are made. The synaptic junctions are very important
for the activity in one nerve cell to excite the neighbouring cell.
Receptor cells are those cells which receive sensory information from their environment.
These help in coding the information thus received into electrochemical pulses
which are responded to by the effector cells. The fine surface membrane which
covers the neuron maintains a difference in chemical constitution between the
neuron's interior and exterior.
The interior of the axon contains a concentration of positively charged potassium
ions. The cellular fluid through which the axon runs contains positively charged
sodium ions. Negatively charged chlorine ions are present in both fluids. The
neuron is stimulated strongly and this stimulation leads to a quick exchange of
ions between the inside and outside of the surface membrane. A pulse of electrical
activity passes along the axon. But energy is not transmitted from one point to
another over the axon. It is only the local electrical activity that moves along
the axon.
A neuron must be sufficiently stimulated for the impulse to be sent along the
fibre. The stimulus thus sent must be increased to the neuron's threshold level.
When the stimulus is sufficiently greater than the level of excitation the neuron
sends a pulse along its axon. The shape and amplitude can be relatively independent
of the intensity of the stimulus. We notice a refractory period, very minute indeed,
but this refractory period is sufficient enough to avoid the production of a new
pulse, regardless of how intense the stimulation is. This is an absolute refractory
period. There is yet another refractory period slightly longer in which the neuron's
threshold level will be higher than the normal level. Neurologists relate these
intervals to the time necessary for the displaced ions to move back to where they
were before the pulse occurred. Notice that this time gap is also necessary for
the completion of an utterance. Note also that we have the phenomena in which
the overlapping of utterances takes place.
The production of pulses by the neuron depends upon the intensity of the stimulus.
There is a limit, however, on the number of pulses which a neuron can produce
each second because of the refractory period. When the nerve fibre reaches its
maximum rate, increase in stimulus activity can have no effect. Several fibres
are fired even for hundred pulses per second. The diameter of the axon decides
the velocity at which a pulse travels along a nerve fibre.
Fig
.2 1.
Cell body 2. Myelin sheath 3. Node of Ranvier
The large nerve fibres of human beings have a layer called the myelin sheath.
(See Fig.2). This sheath is a fatty substance and is an electrical insulator.
This sheath has periodic nodes at which very short lengths of the axon membrane
are exposed. The pulses seem to proceed with greater speed in such nodded fibres.
There seems to be a jump from one node to the other. Though the process and the
organization are not yet fully understood, the noded fibres seem to be a leap
forward in the wiring structure of the nervous system. This is so because the
lower forms of life do not have such noded fibres.
We do not have myelin sheaths for dendrites and nerve endings. Neurologists find
that we transfer activity from one nerve to the other with the help of these.
But again the process is yet to be fully understood. They suggest chemical means
for the transmission process across synapses. The synapses can have either excitatory
or inhibitory function. In the first a pulse reaching a nerve end makes the succeeding
neuron fire. In the second a pulse reaching a nerve end prevents the succeeding
neuron from firing. It is the combined effect of the stimuli that finally determine
the response.
The
central and peripheral systems : Their roles in information processing :
The nervous
system may be divided into central and peripheral subsystems. The brain and the
spinal cord make the central nervous system. The peripheral system consists of
the nerve fibres that connect all the parts of the body with the central nervous
system. The fibres of the peripheral nervous system may be either sensory or motor.
The sensory fibres transmit impulses initiated by external stimuli to the central
nervous system generally. The motor fibres of peripheral nerves conduct the nerve
pulses to the parts of the body to cause appropriate muscular movements. There
are also other fibres of the peripheral system. These fibres go to organs of the
body, such as glands and control the activity of these organs.
The central nervous system is responsible for the coordination and direction of
most human behaviour. The central nervous system sorts out the information brought
by the peripheral nerves, interprets the information and inititates appropriate
action. The central nervous system can also initiate its own action without external
stimuli.
The CNS seems to be organized on some hierarchical lines. The spinal cord is concerned
with the interpretation of elementary information and initiation of elementary
activities such as automatic reflex responses. The brain may be considered as
the high level in this hierarchy (See Fi.3). The medulla oblongata which lies
at the upper end of the spinal cord, controls the reflex mechanisms of the respiratory,
circulatory and digestive systems. The cerebellum receives information about body
position and movement, and muscles and their movement. The central hemispheres
have many deep convolutions. The control many lower functions as well as memory,
consciousness and voluntary activities. The human brain's hemispheres have a great
complexity that has not been found in the brains of animals. The folded surface
of the hemispheres have concentrations of neuron. These concentrations are known
as the cerebral cortex.
As stated earlier, the neural processes involved in the thought prior to speech,
speech production itself, retrieval of speech and so on are poorly understood
even today, though we have now a variety of techniques to obtain information and
conduct research in this area. Anatomy of the nervous system is largely known
through postmortem dissections. This has enabled us to trace the origins and ends
of nerve fibres, to have a good description of the kinds of synapses and the neurons.
Scientists have made observations of the nervous functions using live animals.
The electrodes inserted in different parts of the auditory pathway observe nerve
pulses which result from the acoustic stimulation. Again the electrodes are used
to map the areas of the brain essential for certain functions like hearing, vision,
or motor activity. Certain areas of the brain have been removed and the resultant
activity or lack of it studied. Likewise certain nerves may be cut and the resultant
communication conditions studied. This enables us to know about the functions
of various localized areas of the nervous system. The external electrodes are
used to observe electrical activity. However, major surgery
Fig.
3
1.
Cerebral Hemisphere | 4.
Thalamus |
2.
Cerebral Cortex | 5.
Medulla oblongata |
3.
Cerebellum | 6.
Spinal Cord |
is necessary to observe electrical activity in localized areas and this involves
danger to the subject. Still direct experiments have been conducted on the brain,
as a last resort only on cases whose maladies are severe. And on the basis of
such direct experiments we come to know of the essentiality of certain localized
areas of the cerebral cortex for the production of speech and comprehension. This
indicates that large areas of the brain may be removed without impairing speech
production and comprehension.
A speech production originates from a high level in the central nervous system;
whereas the hearing process begins in the inner ear. The inner ear receives the
signals and these signals are conducted to the sensory centers of the cerebral
cortex. While such signals are transmitted, some information processing may take
place at the synapses. The information is carried on to the central nervous system.
Further there is a complete and complicated feedback loop by which control is
exercised over the peripheral organs. We do not however know in any detail the
what, how and why of the information processing procedures.
It is still a mystery as to how the acoustic signals are perceived by the ear
and then transmitted to the central nervous system. We do not know how our ears
determine the loudness, how they analyze the complex sounds into component tones
and so on. The current view places an emphasis on the inner ear's capacity to
analyze the frequencies of the sound waves.
The hypothalamus, a phylogentically very old part of the brain, is considered
to be the motivational center of the brain. This seems to control several behaviours
and physiological functions, such as sham rage, regulation of body temperature.
The reticular activiating (RAS) is located above the spinal cord and below the
thalamus and hypothalamus. This system seems to be involved in sleeping, wakefulness
and attention. The RAS is stimulated by the sensory information and it relays
this new information or the presence of stimulation to the cortex. This is sent
in the form of attention calls which enable the cortex to get ready to process
the specific information through specific input channel to the cortex. Attention
thus seems to be controlled by the RAS, the destruction of which can make the
animal to tune off completely.
Neurophysiologists suggest that habituation takes place as soon as novelty of
stimulation is lost. Habituatory control perhaps is not the function of higher
brain centers which may concentrate on more novel and significant information.
The distinction between habituatory and non-habituatory information and the relative
importance and novelty of the information may be made either at the periphery
or in the central analyses. Knowledge on this count is scanty once again.
We have
two kinds of views about the neural basis for the retention of learned behaviour
in the dynamic view, the learning situation leads to a continuing electrical activity
in appropriate neural circuits. The memory is related to the persistence of the
continuing electrical activity in the circuits. In the structural view the learning
situation leads to some rather long lasting physical change in the nervous system.
This lasting of the physical change is not dependent upon the original circuits
which brought in the change. Experiments have shown that even when the electrical
activity is reduced or completely eroded, the learning continues to be retained.
As regards the second view, information is scanty and controversial either to
confirm or to reject it. Neurological evidence suggests that the ribonucleic acid
(RNA) is a very strong contender for being the memory molecule.
Neurologists attempt to identify the biochemistry of the brain, seeking answers
for the difference in the learning capabilities of individuals. They try to identify
the chemicals, and their relative spread in the different areas of the brain.
They try to measure the amount of chemicals in neural cells in specific brain
structures and identify the changes they undergo in specific activities undertaken
by the subject. One may manipulate the rearing and learning experiences of animals
with a common background and identify how their brain chemistry has been changed
in the light of experience gained. Further, one may find out how the behaviour
of individuals with different distributions of chemicals vary from each other.
Motor theory of perception and auditory theory of perception :
In the
previous sections we presented some information about the network and the functions
of nerves and the central and peripheral systems with their roles in information
processing. Before we take up the neurological models of behaviour, we would like
to draw the attention of our readers to two approaches to speech perception, viz.,
the motor theory of speech perception and the auditory theory of speech perception.
The current research on the role of motor movements in perception, especially
of speech perception, indicates that a child perceives sound sequences in terms
of his own motor reactions. This motor theory of perception, however, does not
deny the importance of the acoustic events, but gives priority to the motor kinesthetic
feedback loops in the comprehension-use of language. For the motor theory of perception
the proprioceptive impulses are very important. They have their origin in stretch
or tension, receptors in muscles, tendons, joints, and in the vestibular apparatus
of the ear. 'The resulting appreiciation of position, movement, balance and change
in the equilibrium is called proprioception or kinesthesis' (Berry, 1969). Proprioception
is carried on in sensori-motor loops with bi-directional tracts. These sensori-motor
loops are found to be used in comprehension -- use of language. A.M. Lieberman,
the chief proponent of the theory, asserts that the perception of speech sounds
is somehow more closely related to the articulation than to the acoustic stimulus.
Speech is perceived by the articulatory movements and their sensory effects mediate
between the acoustic stimulus and its perception. The articulatory movements that
a listener makes in reproducing the acoustic patterns help in determining the
cues for perception of words. Linguists like Twaddell (Twaddell 1952) also are
of the opinion that listeners classify sound sequences on the basis of motor articulation
rather than acoustic properties. Further some psychologists like Sherry (Cherry
1957) suggest that if there is an order in acoustic and articulatory events, we
perceive the sound sequence only after the neuromuscular patterns of articulation
have been mediated. (For details of this approach, one may like to read A.M. Lieberman's
articles particularly Lieberman, et al 1963; and Lieberman, 1957).
The studies in the auditory perception of speech concentrate on a person's ability
in hearing for speech. In these studies, the attributes of tone, namely, fundamental
frequency, amplitude and duration form the basis of analysis and are found embedded
in the discrimination cues to phonemic sequences of speech. The auditory perception
researchers study also how the different parts of the ear and other systems such
as reticular, thalamus and cortex are involved in the auditory perception. These
studies deal mainly with the temporal management of information from the input.
They aim at explaining how a person's nervous system learns to comprehend and
make use of auditory information. Such studies include several information, namely,
analysis of the rapidly successive bits of information that crowd the auditory
mechanism at the initial stage, initial patterning of the information through
activating and feedback processes, linking up these patterns with other wave patterns
from modalities and modification of wave patterns effected partly by earlier patterns
activated in cortical and subcortical areas. Auditory perception of speech depends
also upon memory, that is, the retained auditory patterns in the nervous system.
The physiological limits set by the auditory apparatus may also be studied in
the auditory perception of speech.
The studies of auditory perception of speech, thus, include both the physical
aspects of sound and the physiological aspects/apparatus for audition. In these
studies, time as a measure is considered to be a very important factor in all
the neural events including frequency variations, duration of input signals, periodicity
into elements, identification and match of elements, and in the production of
speech patterns (Berry, 1969). The auditory perception may be imagined to be taking
place in several phases. Berry (1969) presents three phases in auditory perception
of speech, while agreeing at the same time that such identification and segmentation
of auditory processes of perception are merely a kind of abstraction for the matching
of specific neural activity with the temporal phase. In the first phase activation
of neurons takes place and this requires a chemical mediator which is responsible
for the sensitivity of end organs. Further the change in the end organs should
be transformed into a form of energy capable of discharging the nerve terminals.
In the second phase, the wave pattern makes connections with various parts of
the hearing apparatus. While these connections are being made, modification and
discrimination of the auditory patterns continue. At this stage information from
other modalities, particularly from the reticular system, is available for organizing
and focusing the perceptive field of audition. In the third phase, certain neural
events occur in order to improve the quality of the perceptual processes of audition.
All the systems that are relevant for perception in general pool their resources
in arriving at a comprehensive auditory perception of speech and other events.
At this phase short-term memory (use of retained auditory patterns from earlier
inputs) is utilized. Analysis by synthesis of various number and kinds of information
is the highlight of the auditory perception process in the third phase. In linguistic
terms, ordering and sequencing of syllables, words, phrases and sentences are
achieved in this phase. For detailed information in relation to aspects of auditory
perception of speech, the readers may refer to Fay (1966), Lieberman (1967), Rosenblith
(1961) and Granit (1962).
Neurophysiological models of behaviour : General remarks :
In every society we have a belief in one form or the other that man's intelligence
is a gift given to him by the Almighty. There is also a belief in every society
linking brain and intelligence. The existence of such a belief can be found in
their language inn which the intelligence is referred to as emanating from the
brain. Many societies posit entities called mind, brain and others which are supposed
to control human behaviour in some sense or the other. Clinical neurologists,
neuropsychologists and neurophysiologists-several of them - are and have been
involved in intense research work to identify the exact cerebral localization
for the various mental and physical functions. In fact the clinical neurology
and neuro-physiology are dominated by research activities with the identification
of exact cerebral localization as the main focus. Researchers demanding 'exactitude'
assume that they can find a correlation, perhaps one to one correspondence, between
the part or structure of any size of the brain and specific kinds of behaviour
or functions. For every action and for every function there must be a center,
a nerve or something else in the brain and in finding such correlations depends
our ability to characterize and explain human behaviour. The above assumption
is a historical product and is a direct reflection and extension, in general,
of the reflex theories. Coupled with such a concept of cerebral localizations
is the attempt on the part of the researchers to seek their models in the machines
and mechanisms about which they have an explicit knowledge. Usually a communication
model is constructed and the characteristics of these models are assumed to be
the underlying neural processes etc., on grounds such as communication is not
possible without these characteristics and so on. Thus we have theories based
on hydraulic systems, telephone, radio and others. Such an approach may clarify
the issues and sharpen the focus. At the same tie it can lead to a narrow view
which leads to comparisons, and deductions based on materials which may not have
direct relevance. We must rather study the brain and the behaviour to find out
how the brain works instead of making comparisons. Similarities in such comparisons
may be the product of an oversimplification of the problem of behaviour.
Earlier
we have presented certain anatomical descriptions of the parts of the brain, neurophysiological
processes and indicated even certain localizations of functions in the brain parts.
We will present below three approaches which are in some complementary to each
other in accounting for the neuropsychological organization of human behaviour
including language.
Our behaviour is a product of our nervous system. This fact has been recognized
by psychologists. Many psychologists have used neurophysiological constructs even
as the basis of their theories. Even linguists are no exception to this. However,
there is and has been a very strong tendency to base the explanations mainly on
the observable phenomena. As a result the physiological constructs have been avoided
in several influential psychological theories.
Bloomfield, the most influential among the American structural linguists, opposed
the mentalistic interpretation of language and took a behaviouristic view. Linguists
were in the process of establishing their field as a separate entity and they
succeeded to a large extent in their effort. In this effort and process, the emphasis
has been to avoid the so called psychologizing and neurophysiological assumptions
and implications, even though some makers of modern linguists resorted to neurophysiological
interpretations of the language phenomenon and its underlying mechanisms.
The field
of psychology also has been characterized in the past by the use of certain constructs
which cannot be directly observed. The behaviourists emphasized the need to base
their constructs and explanations on the directly observable phenomena. Coupled
with this movement was the swing towards the separation and establishment of psychology
as an independent field of activity on its own merits.
Though such a demarcation of the fields, and establishment of independent constructs
and techniques are essential for any field to grow and to contribute in a precise
manner to the general body of knowledge, excessive independence can lead to disastrous
results. This is especially so with regard to the relationship between psychology
and neurophysiology. This is true also for the relationship between psychology,
linguistics and neurophysiology.
Hebb's model of behaviour :
Hebb (1949) presents a theory of behaviour, seeking a common ground with the anatomist,
physiologist and neurologist, showing them how the psychological theory is related
to their problems. He finds that one is forced to oversimplify when he deals with
behaviour, because of the scanty knowledge we have about the underlying mechanisms.
This leads one to a kind of concealed mysticism, even when one takes a formalistic
view.
Though psychologists take for granted that the behaviour and neural functions
are perfectly correlated and that the problem of understanding behaviour is the
problem of understanding the total action of the nervous system and vice versa,
Hebb finds, to his regret, that there is a tendency in psychology to stop using
physiological hypothesis. He also finds that many physiologists take the same
position with regard to psychology. One is on a formal and certain ground when
he studies the electrical activity of a well-defined tract in the brain. However,
a question arises as to whether such specific and particular studies alone can
lead to a physiology of the human brain. The parts and their properties may be
discovered in isolation, but the parts may have properties not discernible in
isolation. Perhaps it is necessary to study the brain as a whole, in addition
to a study of the specifics and parts of the brain, to have a clear and realistic
picture of the functioning of the human brain. Hence Hebb calls for learning about
what the parts of the brain do , which belong basically to the physiologist's
field and for relating the human behaviour as far as possible to this knowledge,
which is basically the psychologist's field. He calls for seeing what further
information can be had about how the total brain works from the discrepancy between
actual behaviour and the behaviour which would be predicted from the addition
of what is known about the action of the various parts of the human brain.
The problem
we face in accounting for human behaviour has two facets. From the psychological
view it is a problem of thought. Here we have a process which is not fully controlled
by environmental stimulation and yet it goes along with the environmental stimulation.
From the physiological point of view it is a problem of the transmission of excitation
from sensory to motor cortex. Transmission can be a very complex process involving
time-lag between sensory stimulation and the final motor response. Hebb considers
that both psychology and neurophysiology are not in a position to handle thought
and its corresponding neurophysiological mechanism adequately. One is not in a
position to describe behaviour simply as an interaction directly between sensory
and motor processes. Something intervenes and this something we call thinking.
This thinking must be due to the operation of central processes. Though one is
able to identify the pathways to cortex and the pathways from cortex and also
about the structures that link the two, the complexity of the links is not yet
fully understood. As a result one does not know anything about what goes on between
the arrival of an excitation and its later departure from the motor area of the
cortex. Two types of formula have been proposed to account for the processes.
The first one may be called a switch board theory and the second one may be called
a field theory. In the first type the cells in the sensory system acquire connections
with cells in the motor system. The cortex functions as a telephone exchange.
In the second theory the conception of field is utilized. The cortex is made up
of cells and the sensory control of motor centers depends upon the distribution
of the sensory-excitation, ratios of excitation and not on the locus or the action
of any particular cell. Connections do not control learning. However, Hebb finds
that both these two theories do not account for the delay between stimulation
and response which is characteristic of thought. Intra cerebral events must be
a prolonged one is such a case. But this has not been explained or provided for.
Hebb presents a speculative physiological theory of behaviour with a synthesis
of psychological information. This presents a temporally organized intra cerebral
process with the psychological facts of perception, learning, expectancy, attention
and soon. He suggests that a frequently repeated stimulation leads to the slow
development of a cell assembly. This is a diffuse structure of cells in the cortex
and diencephalons and possibly in the basal ganglia of the cerebrum. This can
act as a closed system delivering facilitation to other such systems and usually
having a specific motor facilitation. The series of such events makes a phase
sequence and this phase sequence is the thought process.
Each assembly action can be aroused by a preceding assembly. It can be aroused
also by a sensory event. The central facilitation issuing from one of these activities
and impinging on the next is the prototype of attention. The adult waking behaviour
is the result of the cortical organization of the slow development of a cell assembly.
There is an alternate intrinsic organization. This occurs in sleep and in infancy
and consists of hypersynchrony in the firing of cortical cells. There is also
disorganization as the assembly depends completely on a fine and delicate timing
which might be disturbed by metabolic changes and sensory events which are not
in accordance with the pre-existent central process. Transitory type of such disturbance
is called emotional disturbance. A chronic disturbance is neurosis or psychosis.
Hebb's theory, as he himself calls it, is a form of connections, one of the switchboard
variety. The connections are not direct connections to and from the cortex. The
connections are there to establish autonomous central activities and these autonomous
central activities form the basis for further learning. Further no nerve cell
or pathway is essential to any habit or perception.
Luria's model of Behaviour :
A.R. Luria of Soviet Union is a great neuropsychologist of all times. His contribution
to an understanding of the organization and disorganization of human behaviour
is of vital importance for many reasons. His experiments were conducted mainly
on human beings, using that medium, namely, language which defines man as man.
Luria identified and established a method for neuropsychology to probe and penetrate
the underlying mechanisms not manifest in overt behaviour.
Luria argues that the complex mechanisms of organization and disorganization of
human behaviour cannot be explained as a simple play of neurophysiological processes.
The causes of the affective processes should not be sought in the peripheral apparatus,
but in the central. No phenomena of elementary neurodynamics can explain the configurations
of integrated behaviour specific for the human beings as social subjects. Perhaps
even the elementary neurodynamics may be comprehended only through an analysis
of the culturally created psychological functions which include the involved behaviour
of work, speech and complex indirect operations.
Luria does not attempt to deduce the laws of higher activity from simple neuro
dynamical processes. Neither the laws of dynamics of tendency nor the conditions
of reflex connections can throw light on the most complicated forms of human behaviour.
Only a careful description of the specific systems of behaviour produced in the
process of socio-historical development alone will help us in comprehending the
higher neurodynamics.
Luria attacks the tendency to introduce naïve concepts and analogies with
artificial things to explain the nervous system. The telephone system was considered
the basic theory of nervous activity. Related to this is the concept of the structure
of neuropsychology, which posits a series of separate self-sufficient mechanisms.
These mechanisms were assumed to have been arranged with the help of the connecting
excitations and inhibitions.
In the above view, the whole nervous apparatus is assumed to consist of separate
neurons and the brain is assumed to be nothing more than a centralization of these
neurons and their conduits which connect the neurons. Accepting this assumption
means that the laws of behaviour must be found in those laws which hold for the
individual neurons. The behaviour has to be understood merely as a preservation
of equilibrium between the separate apparatus of the nervous system. The pathological
conditions which one notices must be deduced as a destruction of this equilibrium.
This kind of analogy leads to naïve postulations. This makes one to postulate
that the elementary processes of excitation and inhibition are basic, founding
every nerve cell and carried throughout the whole organism. This also leads one
to believe that at a given moment only certain cells of the nervous system are
in a state of excitation and others in a state of inertness. Other cells are in
an inhibitory condition.
Luria suggests that the process of excitation and inhibition are elementary processes
and this occurs in the isolated nerve preparation. Luria asks whether we can express
all the forms of organization or disorganization of human behaviour in terms of
elementary inhibition and excitation. He finds that this conception does not extend
adequately to the whole phenomena. The behaviour cannot be explained as an equilibrium
of the separate systems. The disease of any of the mechanisms causes general changes
which can be understood only from a most complicated functional, reciprocity of
the internal behaviour. The conceptions which one needs for an understanding of
the characteristic mechanisms of complicated human behaviour requires that we
take into account the whole organization of behaviour, with its structure and
dynamics. The organization is sought in a functional correlation of the systems
because these systems are not combined in an accidental way but as very definite
parts into an integrated functional structure. The parts are functionally equal.
Certain systems are meant for governing and regulating while others for supporting
and exacting one for another function.
Luria finds that the organization and disorganization of human behaviour, conditions,
laws and formulations are the most important problems of psycho-biology. There
are some general laws operative in the organization of behaviour. These general
laws depend upon the inclination of some special vital forces. The organization
we find in adult human behaviour is the product of a fairly complicated and long
development. This development precedes in such a way that it dominates the primitive
laws and not in the way that it becomes the simple representations in new stages.
By taking a developmental view we can arrive at an understanding of the activity
of the human personality. The development will include the new regulating systems
intended to overcome the primitive forms of behaviour and transfer them to a new
and more systematized organization. In this development the question of age does
not play the leading role. The primitive forms of organization of behaviour is
characterized by the sub-cortical activity. This activity is transferred into
the process of the highest development. There is a conflict between the development
of newly regulated systems and the primitive sub-cortical activity. The conflict
creates all the new forms of organization. The higher cortical mechanism plays
an active role in the formation of the new organization. Because of this participation
the higher cortical mechanism is included in the regulating systems.
The development of a child involves several processes. First there is a general
development of regulations and this begins with the primitive aspects of instinctive
capabilities. With the development of higher psychological mechanisms the most
complicated forms of control of behaviour is achieved. Such an achievement begins
with the complicated organic mechanism and with the higher cultural systems conditioning
new forms of organization.
There are two general groups of approaches for the study of the motor functions
of the human being. In the first group scholars study the development of motor
movement co-ordination, motor formulae and their destruction caused by certain
diseases of the nervous system. This study concentrates on the motor function
as a physiological process. This can conceal symptoms underlying the disturbance.
In the second group the motor activity of the human being is only a means to study
the complicated psychological and physiological processes. The structure of the
movement is studied as a reflection of certain changes concealed from immediate
observation. For the scholars who pursue this type of research the motor activity
is significant in so far as this is connected with the psychological changes.
Such an approach makes the investigation more complicated and yet infinitely more
significant than the previous one. The main problem is to differentiate the motor
changes which are the product of the psychological influence from those resulting
from the organic peculiarities. Luria's research is more closely allied with the
second group. He is only very slightly interested in the motor activities of the
subject per se.
Luria suggests that we study voluntary movements in the analysis of behaviour.
The voluntary movements are not less regular, not less suitable than the reflexes.
He argues in favour of the combination of the central and the motor activities
in one functional system. If we combine we can record that the central change
is necessarily reflected in the motor system. What we have here is a united whole.
The central part is concealed from direct study and yet the motor functions can
be objectively registered. This method of the basic combination of the central
and motor activities is called the combined motor method. Luria steered his researches
toward an understanding of the neuro-psychological processes through the combined
method. The subject is asked to reply to a word given him by the first thought
which enters his mind. While doing so he is asked to press the finger of the right
hand of the receiver of an apparatus lying in front of him. Such a process stimulates
in the subject two systems of activity. These systems are connected with each
other so closely that they are set into motion by two simultaneously occurrence
of the activities of one and the same process. When we ask a subject to answer
a given word by another one, his central process is excited in a very complicated
manner, and this is very close to the speech system. When we analyze the result
psychologically we can in some cases find its associative process, in some cases
its primitive fate and in others 'its reintegration with the origin of the whole
image of the details contained in the word, or the production of some other details
entering together with what is represented in the word-stimulus into one and the
same formation'. In this process we evoke definite and a complicated neuro-dynamic
process which cannot be observed immediately and which leads after a certain period
to the speech response. The language response is connected to the motor reaction
of the hand and uniting those two into a single process, Luria finds that we can
estimate 'the actual changes in this obscured process as necessarily reflected
in a clearly defined process and we see that the differences in the neurodynamic
structure of the central process are reflected in the evident differences of structure
in the motor curve'. Luria suggests that every short fluctuation and every tendency
to a speech response, and, even more so, every marked affective disorganized character
of the central process does not remain without influence on the structure of the
compounded motor reaction ; and analyzing it, we have at hand a very objective
means for drawing conclusions concerning the structure of the internal neurodynamic
process'.
The psychologists and neurophysiologists who are interested in identifying the
hidden processes between movement of stimulation and the movement of clear response
have to surmount many obstacles. Failure in characterizing the underlying mechanisms
is due in no small measure to this almost insurmountable obstacle. We have to
find out whether the evident response is indeed the result of the undisturbed
and uninterrupted process or the process is connected with the struggle of the
direct tendencies. There can be internal suppressions. Even an honest subject
can inadvertently or because of his socio-cultural inhibition may suppress his
original response and give another one in its place. Luria is aware of these difficulties
and suggests that the combined method can be so manipulated that we can objectively
study only the central process. In a number of experiments the subjects are sought
to answer with the first word they thought of. In these experiments they are asked
to first give the answer and then press the button.
What we have in behaviour is a union of internal processes and its resultant manifestation.
In the combined method what we get in essence is an active union which gives us
a unified acting structure. The unified acting structure consists of both the
obscure and externally observed symptoms. The changes initiated in one side is
reflected inevitably in the other side. An analysis of this active union enables
us to study the character of the highest psychological processes.
Luria, thus, is a psychologist, a neuropsychologist using mainly the linguistic
utterances as basic items/tools for the analysis of underlying mechanisms. He
occupies a unique position among the psychologists and others, because of his
use of linguistic items and lucid exposition of underlying mechanisms. In addition
to using linguistic items as tools of his analysis, Luria has conducted several
experiments to identify the underlying mechanisms of language performance. We
give below an experiment he made to elucidate the conflict of the language.
We find
that various languages use very different words for one and the same set of contents.
A person speaking in a language creates a definite setting in that language which
loses its forms when transferred to another language. But the expressed contents
remain completely untouched as far as the motor innervations are concerned. Luria
in this experiment aims at brings into collision such language settings, evoking
a conflict of two very complicated structural systems. He wants to investigate
the neurodynamics of the conflicts that may result in a person in such a transfer.
Subjects who know the languages equally well are given words from both these languages
and are asked to answer by the first word they thought of in every case. But the
answer must be given in the language of the given stimulus in every case. Thus
the experiment includes the conditions of the conflict. The conflict, however,
is not connected with the content of the word. There are two parallel series of
the experiment. This is to ensure that the relations we obtain are not connected
with the ignorance of the foreign language but with the factor of the sudden change
of the setting. In the first series five foreign words are scattered among twenty
five Russian ones. In the second series, five Russian words are scattered among
twenty five foreign words. A basic setting is created when the first eleven words
of each series are from the basic language of the series. The stimuli words themselves
are of equal difficulty. The results indicate that one problem is quickly transferred
to another language setting, after the setting to that language is created. It
is found that crucial reactions occur with a distinct slowing and considerable
disturbance in the accompanying motor system. Associations do not create the disturbance
as the subjects know both the languages equally well. The disturbance is due to
the position of the critical words in the series. This is revealed in the second
series which has only five Russian words scattered among the twenty five foreign
words. The critical Russian words call out more inhibited reaction than even the
foreign critical words in the Russian series. This is because we have two processes
- a transfer to a new setting and a removal of the former setting. We meet with
disturbance in the accompanying motor activity in cases connected in one way or
another with the conflict of the settings.
The sudden appearance of the new language setting produces a shock which may disturb
the receptory activity or cause a conflict in the motor system. The receptory
failure is due to the sudden change of setting. The reaction to word depends upon
the context in which the word is used. It is found that when the context is altered,
perception can be difficult, the word inaudible or it changes its sounds and so
on. In the second case, a setting is created to react to definite language. This
setting is not removed during a sudden transfer and continues, when the transfer
collides with this tendency to continue. A conflict in the linguistic motor system
is brought out. The subject begins to give the equivalent in the basic language
instead of working himself to think and give another word. Such a tendency can
even be extended to non-critical words in the series. These processes indicate
the conflict the subject is undergoing. The conflict in the motor activity is
clearly shown in the graphic curves. The conflict causes a shock of the higher
speech processes in the subject. This is accompanied by a breaking of the functional
barrier and the emancipation of the motor area from the connected organized process.
The inhibition of speech reaction we find usually in such shocks and transfers
is found to be due to the direct transfer of the excitation to the motor sphere.
Further the conflict caused by the critical stimuli leaves traces which continue
for some time and gets extinguished only gradually, influencing all the while
the reactions to other stimuli.
Luria, like his late colleague Vygotsky, recognizes that speech can be used for
auto-stimulation and organization of behaviour. Speech is the means of regulating
and organizing the external world. It is an agent for organizing the behaviour.
It is an agent for planning further actions; it enables the human to avoid subordination
by direct optical means. Organizing the behaviour begins with speech auto-commands
and ends with the complex forms of judgement. Luria finds that the mechanisms
of the substitution of the primitive process by cultural signals is the most important
factor in the control of human behaviour.
Finally, Luria is skeptical about the attempts which localize the intricate psychological
process in parts of the brain and the nervous system. It is easier to think in
terms of things than in terms of process and this is perhaps the reason for continuing
attempts to localize the psychological processes. It must be assumed that the
acquisition of speech, the use of instruments and the transition of the new cultural
forms of organization of the individual behaviour, all achieved in social setting,
will also change the structure of the psychophysiological processes. These changes,
the functional changes, are much more important, for in several cases. Luria claims,
the use of these compensates for serious defects in the morphological structure
of the nervous apparatus. There may be a definite organization of the cerebral
apparatus underlying complex psychological phenomena, but there need be no specific
morphological new formulations for every peculiarity. 'Cultural behaviour does
not require a new brain morphology, and the brain of a savage may be morphologically
identical with that of a member of Academy of Sciences.'
Lashley's model of behaviour :
Lashley's contributions are very significant of an understanding of the organization
and control of behaviour. Lashley did not use linguistic utterances as his major
tool for the analysis of the underlying mechanisms of human behaviour. He relied
upon surgery to demonstrate the untenability of the localization concept. In Lashley's
words : 'In experiments extending over the past thirty years I have been trying
to trace conditioned reflex paths through the brain or to find the locus of specific
memory traces. The results for different types of learning have been inconsistent
and often mutually contradictory in spite of confirmation by repeated tests' (Lashley
1960). Both Lashley and Luria arrived at some similar conclusions and findings
through the use of different approaches, methods and tools.
Lashley has chosen rats and monkeys as his subjects. In a few cases chimpanzees
were also his subjects. He followed two lines of approach. In the first one which
is a behavioural type the analysis of the excitations was done. These excitations
are actually associated wit reactions in learning and are effective in the elicitation
of the learned reactions. Lashley analyzed the associated reactions also. This
approach defined the patterns of nervous activity at the receptor and effector
levels. The second approach is by surgical destruction of parts of the brain.
Lashley trained animals in wide ranging tasks from direct sensory motor associations
to the solution of difficult problems. Portions of the brain and associated links
were cut or removed. The resultant effect of these surgical operations were measured.
When the experiments were over the brain parts were sectioned and the extent of
damage reconstructed from serial sections.
In an experiment monkeys were trained to open various latch boxes. After this
training their motor areas were removed resulting in a temporary paralysis. Yet,
after eight to twelve weeks the animals became capable of opening the boxes. The
monkeys did not have access to the training boxes. When sufficiently recovered
they did not exhibit even exploratory movements. Hence learning must be considered
as the specific localization of the whole of cerebellum.
As regards the role of cortex and the connections to it, Lashley's experiments
showed that neither the motor cortex nor the connections are indispensable for
the transmission of the conditioned reflex pattern. Lashley trained rats on the
maze and then made knife cuts through the cortex and underlying fibres. This disconnected
or cut through different functional areas. After recovery the animals did not
show any significant loss of the learned behaviour. The general assumption in
the field is that there are conditioned reflex paths connecting sense organ and
the motor cortex via association areas. Lashley's experiments, however, disprove
involvement of the motor areas. Further his experiments show that the memory trace
cannot be localized anywhere within the nervous system. It is perhaps true that
limited regions are essential for learning or retention of a particular activity.
But even if there is a limited region, the parts within the region seem to be
functionally equivalent.
The frontal, parietal, occipital and temporal areas of the cortex are considered
as associative areas because of their anatomic relations and these are considered
as the store house of images of sensations. Lashley suggests that these associative
areas are concerned more with the modes of organization than with any specific
memory storage. Further these areas seem to be concerned with the general facilitation
or maintenance of the level of vigilance. Destruction of these does not lead to
amnesia but only to difficulties in the performance of tasks involving abstractions
and generalization.
As the parts of a region have equivalent capacity, there may be multiple representation
of an event in a region. The pattern of excitation leads to a pattern of activity
and this pattern of activity is fully reduplicated throughout an entire functional
area, 'much as the surface of a liquid develops an interference pattern of spreading
waves when it is disturbed at several points'. Further, Lashley suggests that
all the cells of the brain must be assumed to be in constant activity. There is
no cell in excess which is reserved as the storage of memories. He would argue
that one should not seek for the trace of any activity merely as an isolated connection
between the sensory and motor elements. The spatial and temporal axes of the nervous
system which underlie the behaviour must be considered as the basis even for the
trace of any activity.
The two concepts which Lashley formulated have been already indicated in the foregoing
paragraphs. These are the concepts of equipotentiality and mass action. 'Equipotentiality
of parts is the capacity of any intact part of a functional area to carry out,
with or without reduction in efficiency, the functions that are lost by the destruction
of other parts'. This is 'not an absolute property but is subject to the law of
mass action whereby the efficiency of performance of an entire complex function
may be reduced in proportion to the extent of the brain injury within an area
whose parts are not more specialized for one component of the function than for
another' (Lashley 1960).
Lashley was a contributor to and adherent of the gestalt psychology. He believed
that what the organism perceives is the total pattern and the behaviour is a series
of conditioned reflexes. It is tempting to follow the association - connectionistic
theories, especially after the discovery of neurons and the synapses. But no simple
connectionism can explain the human behaviour and the neurophysiological mechanisms.
Localization does not solve the problem of how a part functions in a complex system.
Specificity is perhaps there in some measure in the visual area, but it is very
much less elsewhere. "Mind" must not be sought in localization as an
integrated interaction of processes reflected in an organization of its own. The
mental phenomenon must be subjected to an analysis as complete and detailed as
that which is being made of neural activities. Only as progress is made in such
an analysis and as the picture of the brain's activities is completed, will it
be possible to make significant correlations between the two organized systems.
Meanwhile, there is no logical or empirical reason for denying the possibility
that the correlation may eventually show a complete identity of the two organizations
(Lashley 1960).
Lashley considered that the temporal organization of behaviour is the most fundamental
and troublesome problem in neurology. (Even now it continues to be so, not only
in neurology, but also in psychology and linguistic semantics and conceptualizations).
A solution may be found, according to Lashley, if we study the translation of
temporal orders of action into spatial patterns and also the reverse in the brain.
In such cases the after-discharge of the sensory excitations persist with a spatial
distribution in the brain. Language may represent the characteristic integrative
functions of the cerebral cortex, but temporal integration is found in all our
behaviour. In the associative chain theory an element in the chain excites the
next element. When applied to language this approach loses its ground. In a sentence,
the order of occurrence of words, one may think, represents the real order of
associations of one word with the next. But words in a sentence can have associations
not only with their immediate neighbour but also with the remote ones. The different
positions of the word "right" in the sentence 'the Millwright on my
right thinks it right some conventional rite symbolize the right of every man
to write as he pleases' are determined by the meanings which the positions in
relation to other words denote, but those meanings are given by other associations
than those with the words in the spoken sentence'. Thus 'the individual items
of the temporal series do not in themselves have a temporal 'valence' in their
associative connections with other elements. The order is imposed by some other
agent.
.The order in which the fingers of the musician fall on the keys
or fingerboard is determined by the signature of the composition. This gives a
set which is not inherent in the association of the individual movements,' (Lashley
1960).
We find that one can easily change the form of expression of the idea and that
different word orders may be employed to express the same thought. These indicate
that the temporal integration is not inherent in the preliminary organization
of the idea. In fact all the elements of an idea must be considered co-temporal.
This precludes the possibility that the intention to act or the idea to be expressed
determines the serial order. Lashley suggests that the mechanism 'which determines
the serial activation of the motor units is relatively independent both of the
motor units and of the thought structure'. The evidence for this is found in the
mistakes of order, and the slips and interferences which occur in writing and
speaking. Then how does the order come into practice?
Lashley suggests that prior to the internal or overt composition of sentence,
a whole range of word units is partially activated or readied. Syntax is then
impressed on the acts as they occur. There are three things in so far as language
is concerned. In the first place we must identify the activation of expressive
elements. As said earlier, these elements do not contain the temporal relations.
In the second place we must identify the characteristics of the determining tendency.
In the third place, we should consider the syntax of the act-'on habitual order
or mode of relating the expressive elements; a generalized pattern or schema of
integration which may be imposed upon a wide range and a wide variety of specific
acts'. The essential problem of the serial order thus is : 'the existence of generalized
schemata of action which determine the sequence of specific acts, acts which in
themselves or in their associations seem to have no temporal valence' (Lashley
1960). As far as language is concerned, the understanding of speech and the production
of speech may involve essentially the same problems. The performance and reaction
to a speech event demand that we postulate an after effect or after discharge
of the sensory compounds for a significant time following stimulation.
As regards
the integration of temporal and spatial systems Lashley suggests that 'the same
cells in the visual cortex participate in a great variety of activities. Practically
all of the cells of the area must be fired by every visual stimulation, and these
same cells must be the ones which retain the visual memories. The conclusion follows
that differential responses depend upon the pattern of cells which are excited
in combination. The visual cortex is a network of cells of short axon without
long interconnections between its parts or with other cortical areas. Its integrative
functions are an expression of the properties of such a network. The same conception
must be applied to other cortical areas. There are, of course, long association
tracts in the cortex'. But Lashley finds that these long associations may be only
skeletal structures and that they do not have any special function. The major
integrative functions must be assumed to be carried out by the network of cells
of short axon. He argues in favour of analyzing the properties of such networks
of cells to understand the mechanisms of the cerebral cortex. All the cells in
the network must be considered as in constant excitation and are firing; this
happens subject to their recovery from the refractory state. Because of this excitation
state, mutual interference of circuits takes place and this results in complicated
patterns. These complicated patterns get stabilized in the absence of differential
stimulation. What happens at any particular point in the system, as at an efferent
neuron, not of the transmission of impulses over a restricted path, of which that
efferent cell forms a link. 'Perhaps one will be able to isolate the parts of
the system, but the isolated parts are influenced by multiplicity of effects,
for which no trace may be found in the isolated preparation of an isolated part'.
Language disorders :
if we assume that psycholinguistics is concerned with the symbolic activity of
human beings, it is all the more important that in such a study the destructional
processes of this activity find a crucial place. The psychobiologists like Luria
have demonstrated that the study of the destructional processes actually contribute
to an understanding of normal human behaviour. A contrast is available, the factors
in several cases can be isolated and one can reconstruct the normal behaviour,
in the process identifying the relative importance of the factors and the structures
that go into the composition of normal human behaviour. Furthermore the analysis
of the destructional processes have immediate applicational value in ameliorating
the unfortunate conditions of the suffering humanity.
Speech and Hearing is a very special field to which several disciplines contribute
their theories, research methods and findings. There are speech disorders which
require surgical and clinical operations. But these operations need not lead to
the establishment of normal speech. In some cases they do not definitely lead
to normal speech. Hence the importance and the need for the speech and hearing
expertise apart from the expertise of surgical and clinical fields, psychology
and neurophysiology, etc. the speech and hearing expert acquires a knowledge of
the neurophysiology of speech pathologies. He acquires a competence in the psychological
analysis of the speech disorders. He can appreciate the role of other psychological
factors involved in the maintenance and destruction of speech. He acquires a sound
knowledge of the physical aspects of sounds both at the production and auditory
levels. He has a knowledge of linguistics, language history of the patients etc.,
which enable him to diagnose a patient linguistically and set before himself formal
linguistic objectives.
Speech pathology is the study of abnormalities of speech-speech disorders. There
are certain social conventions, neurophysiological bases and psychological reasons
for considering someone's speech behaviour at pathological, as speech defects.
If a person's voice is not loud enough, as demanded in normal discourses of a
community, his speech is considered a defective speech. Inaccurate articulations
which are wholly does not follow the rules of his language, which results in his
speech not being understood by the speakers of the same language the person's
speech is considered a discovered one. Some people do have, from the point of
view of listeners, unpleasant voice. Their speech, because of some reasons not
fully explainable, is also considered as disordered. We have some intrinsic notions
about the quality of speech in terms of its rate of delivery, rhythm, pitch, loudness,
timbre, some special sounds with regard to age of persons and their sex. When
such expected notions are violated by the 'peculiarity' of individual's speech,
then we consider it as defective. Even the accompanying gestures, grimaces, postures
and additional noises can lead to someone's speech being considered as a disordered
one. In essence, in a defective speech the listener's attention is distraced from
the subject matter of the discourse to its mechanical mode of delivery.
The speech
and hearing expert does not consider every speech defect a disorder. If the defects
stem from neurophysiological, physiological and anatomical bases, they will be
considered as disorders.
The literature on conditions that facilitate acquisition and performance of normal
speech does not indicate firm grounds, which help speech facility. However, some
experts consider that better physical strength, early establishment of right handedness,
type of intelligence (humanistic, artistic and linguistic talents) and tendency
to appreciate and enjoy music help the speech facility. Acquisition and performance
of speech are not facilitated if the individual has poor physical strength, no
clear establishment of right handedness, tendency to acquire, appreciate and enjoy
mathematics and sciences (Q-type of intelligence) and lack of interest in music.
The role and function of the above in the development and maintenance of speech
facility are quite controversial. Speech defects are found in both groups, although
certain forms of speech disorders such as stuttering are found more among the
male population.
The speech disorders may have their bases in the central and peripheral nervous
systems. However, the speech and hearing experts insist that one should not jump
to conclusion as to the structural bases of any disorder. A proper investigation
may show that a disorder is due simply to psychological factors and that it does
not require surgical and clinical operations. Hence a cautious approach taking
a comprehensive view of the etiological conditions is absolutely necessary.
Voice
disorders :
It is indeed difficult to characterize a voice as a disordered one purely on objective
criteria. Certain social and cultural conventions play a vital role in deciding
upon the quality of voice. Generally we consider a voice as defective if it has
no adequate loudness, clearness of tone, when its pitch does not match the expectations
for the sex and the age, when there is no proper movement of pitch to be followed
in a language. Such vocal defects are called dysphonia or aphonia. In dysphonia
we have vocal defects and yet one retains the voice. In aphonia one loses the
voice.
It would be interesting indeed to study how social and cultural conventions characterize
a voice as pleasant, unpleasant, mellifluous and so on. When a voice lacks vibrato,
one may call it metallic, flat or hard etc. Inappropriate pitch levels to the
age and sex may invite a characterization as high, shrill, etc. irregular vibrato
leads one to characterize a voice as tremulous and so on. A thin voice is that
which lacks adequate loudness. A husky voice is that which lacks clearness of
tone.
There can be structural, neuropathological and emotional disorders and improper
vocal habits can also contribute to dysphonia.
In maintaining the quality of voice the glottis plays a very significant role.
The glottis should have properly formed edges which can be brought close together.
When the edges are brought close together it should allow the escape of any air
only when brought about by chest pressure. Further they should be brought together
in such a way that it would allow the free movement of the bands when vibrated
by the air stream. In a normal situation the throat and the mouth are kept open
enough to achieve proper resonance of the tone. When these are not obtained, we
get defective speech based on the deformities of the glottis. The deformities
do not allow for proper flow of air as demanded.
Anatomically there may be some firm of growths on the cords which lead to improper
approximations. When the bands show active pathology, the expert does not give
voice training. When the bands show irremediable irregularities, no surgery or
medicine can help. Voice sensory-motor control of the voice through ear training
drills. They may be encouraged to learn to adjust the larynx. Relief may be extended
to people whose bands have irregularities as a result of the misuse of voice.
Persons such as singers, auctioneers, and public speakers belong to this category
and can be treated successfully.
Another phenomenon which is often considered as dysphonia is the carry over of
the high pitched voice of the childhood into adolescence and even adulthood. There
are many psychological factors that induce the boy to retain his high pitched
voice. Ultimately he gives up the high-pitched voice and acquires an adult voice.
When the boy reaches puberty the glottis becomes bigger and lengthened. In this
process the boy's voice gets a lower pitch, almost one half of the pubescent rate
of frequency. Several boys, however, do not make the grade and continue to prefer
the high-pitched voice, though they have had an option to adopt either the high-pitched
or the low-pitched voice. This may be due to the premature setting in of the pubescence
and/or the unusual lengthening of the vocal cords. The premature development leads
to the emergence of a sort of husky voice not appropriate to his age level and
not found among his mates. To avoid embarrassment the boy may choose the high
pitched voice which sets in so heavily in his speech habits that he cannot give
it up even after the entry into adolescence and adulthood. The therapy will be
a psycho-therapy, kindling in the individual a desire to change his voice. Intensive
training is followed.
Cleft palate is a congenital deformity. There may be a hiatus along the central
seam of the palate. Or there may be a gap on the alveolar border and the related
structure of the lip. The first one is cleft palate and the second one is usually
called a harelip or cleftlip. One must first establish a control over the opening
into the nasal chamber. Only then sufficient pressure can be built up in the mouth
to produce sounds such as plosives, fricatives and sibilants. The therapists will
try to eliminate excessive nasality and sharpen the discriminations between the
consonants. He will also try to eliminate the usually accompanying movements and
sounds, the patient has developed as substitutions.
In another speech disorder obstructional of the nasal passage causes the patient
to produce corresponding consonant stops for the nasals. The hypertrophy of the
pharyngeal tonsil can lead to this condition. The patient tends not to use the
nasapharyngeal sphincter, as he is able to build up air pressure in the mouth
cavity without using the sphincter. And this leads him to develop the habit of
speaking without involving the velum. This nasal obstruction can be removed by
surgical and clinical operations to certain extent. However, such operations are
only a step forward in the actual treatment of the disorder. Intensive speech
training is necessary to bring in normalcy. The speech habits acquired earlier
can be eliminated only by intensive speech training. Complete elimination is not
guaranteed.
In addition to the above disorders based on structural deformities, the experts
include the following also : openbite in which the upper and lower lip incisors
do not meet each other. This leads to the defective nature of the fricative sounds
produced ; short chin causes incorrect articulations of bilabials and sibilants.
In prognathic mandible condition, the patient has difficulty with the control
of his jaws. He must adjust his speech habits for the production of fricative
sounds. A narrow and pointed roof of the mouth can also lead to the defective
production of sibilants.
Stuttering
:
In stuttering we have muscular spasms which interfere with speech. The spasms
can be of repetitive (clonic) or continuous (tonic nature). The vocalization and
articulation of speech are repetitions and these consist of hesitations. The stutterer
gives a feeling that he is engaged in a struggle to overcome the spasms. Such
struggles increase in social tensions. The spasms can spread to other muscles
also in acute and advanced cases of stuttering. The stutterer thus has a laboured
and tense voice, because of his muscular hypertonicity. The stuttering is found
more among the boys than among the girls. The incidence is high in the age range
of 5 to 10 years. There is rather a rapid decrease as puberty age is approached.
Perhaps the higher incidence of stuttering among the males may be ascribed to
the comparatively late development of language in the males. Stuttering can be
acquired socially, through heredity or by imitation. The stutterer's blood seems
to contain more animal sugar than the blood of the non-stutterer and they are
found more often in families which have multiple births. They are also found more
often in families which have left-handers. Further, stutterers are found more
often in families which are susceptible for allergies.
Stuttering can be caused by the anxiety a child develop when his clumsy speech
is ridiculed as stuttering by his playmates, parents and other contacts. It may
be also a hysterical phenomenon to gain the attention of parents and others. It
may be a result of sexual immaturity and/or a phenomenon seeking compensation
for fear, etc. Experts consider that stuttering may be a result of conflicts of
control between the right and the left cerebral hemispheres. It may be due also
to the delayed development of the myelin sheath of the nervous system. It is found
that the myelin sheath develops and grows faster in girls than in boys.
The jerky
and involuntary speech, called chorea speech, is found among the girls. Stuttering
occurs usually before puberty and the chorea speech occurs usually after puberty.
The chorea speech is of repetitive nature.
Bulbar polio is due to the damage caused to the peripheral nerves which control
the muscles of articulation. One notices stiffness of the neck, fever, muscular
pain and severe head ache. The speech disturbance is noticed only after the sickness
with the above symptoms runs into a full course. Bulbar polio can disturb the
already formed speech habits and it can also arrest further development. The speech
disorders which have their bases in the central nervous system include the following
: encephalitis, neurosyphilis, chronic subdural hematoma, intra cranial aneurysm,
cerebral abscess and glioma. Encephalitis is caused by the inflammation of the
brain which usually occurs from the second to fourth decade. Articulation as well
as the function of language are disturbed. Syphilis in the blood stream causes
the neurosyphilis. The speech is disturbed by dysarthria as well as by other emotional
and aphasic disorders. In chronic subdural hematoma, a cyst of blood tumor occurs
under the covering of the brain, which may press the brain down and cause a paralysis.
This cause disturbance in the articulation of speech as well as in the use of
language. The cerebral abscess is like a boil which presses upon the tissues of
the brain. Glioma is a type of tumor which can cause articulatory incoordinations.
Aphasia is the term we often use to denote the impairment of the faculty of using
or understanding language, both spoken and written. Linguistic impairments can
be congential or non-congential. Infantile aphasia is classified into four major
types. These are aphasia proper in which there is a deficit of spoken language.
This is often called motor aphasia. In the auditory aphasia there is deficit of
the hearing of the language. In alexia the patient is unable to read the language.
In agraphia, the patient is unable to write the language. The patient is unable
to read the language not because of impairment in his vision. The patient is unable
to write the language not because of paralysis of fingers. It is the faculty which
underlies language performance that must be considered as the basis of the aphasic
conditions. Paralysis of the tongue, peripheral deafness, blindness and the paralysis
of the fingers are not considered as factors which decide the description of a
disorder as aphasia. The etiology of aphasia is traced generally to the underdevelopment
and damage of language centers in the cerebrum. Feeblemindedness can result in
aphasoid which is characterized by paucity of vocabulary and defective syntax.
In infantile schizophrenia, the patient has a tendency to withdraw inwardly from
his social contacts. He may understand what is said to him. Yet he may have no
desire to reply to what is spoken. These two cases are also considered wrongly
as constituting aphasia.
Deficiencies
of hearing also lead to communicative disorders. A child may not be able to receive
and comprehend what is told to him. He may have also difficulty in monitoring
his own speech. There are three kinds of deficiencies of hearing. Technically
speaking an individual is considered as a deaf person if he has been unable to
hear speech from birth or early infancy. This person has no linguistic memory
or impression at all. There is no habit of any oral communication. We consider
an individual as a deafened person if he once heard speech in a normal fashion,
but is now unable to hear it. We consider an individual as hard of hearing if
he can hear speech but his efficiency in hearing is lower than the normal standards.
A deaf child as defined above has severe impairments both in his reception and
production of speech. The rate of impairment in the deafened child depends upon
the degree of deafening of the child. When a child is hard of hearing, he has
difficulty in understanding what he hears. He has no difficulty, however, in monitoring
his own speech. There are three kinds of deafness on the basis of etiological
factors. These are mechanical or conduction deafness in which the impairment is
due to defects in the receiving and transmitting portions in the canal of the
outer ear, in the ear drum or middle ear; cochlear deafness in which the impairment
is related to the transmission of mechanical sounds into neutral stimuli and/or
to the sensory end organs of the acoustic nerve, and, nerve deafness in which
there is defect in the acoustic nerve or in the efferent nerves that carry signals
from the cochlea to the auditory centers of the cerebrum. There can be also some
non-organic deafness related to psychogenic factors.
Perception and conduction deafness are the forms of reduced acuity. The patient
produces his speech in a subdued volume in the conduction deafness whereas he
produces shouts in the perception deafness hears his own voice louder than it
really is. He feels that others are speaking in low voice. Hence he begins to
reduce the volume.
Linguists' view of disorders :
we have thus far presented the neurophysiology of speech, three approaches to
the description and explanation of the neurophysiology and psychology of speech
and have given certain kinds of speech disorders from the speech and hearing experts'
point of view. In this section we present the language disorders from the linguists'
view point.
Linguists may also consider the neurophysiological bases of speech in order to
characterize the speech disorders. However, they can describe speech disorders
even without reference to the neurophysiological, psychological and other bases.
They consider the speech of a child as delayed if the development of speech in
that child is not commensurate with the normal development of the chronological
age. When the children are not in a position to follow and produce the patterns
of the language while learning the language, their speech may be described as
disordered. These disorders of speech can occur in all the language levels a linguist
posits for language. Thus one can have disorders at the phonological level, at
the morphological and syntactic levels and also at the semantic level. The disorders
can be looked at again from the point of view of language skills. Thus the disorders
may be related to listening, speaking, reading and writing skills.
At the phonological level the speech is considered defective if a child does not
follow the phonological patterns, omitting certain sounds, submitting certain
others for the original sounds of a language, and also distorting the sounds of
a language. At the morphological and syntactic levels the child may have difficulty
in comprehending the discourses. The child may have normal hearing capacity and
may understand the single words quite well. But he may not be able to produce
the patterns correctly. He may miss the sequence of words; he may omit words and
employ incorrect morphological constructions. At the semantic level he may have
difficulty in comprehending even the normal vocabulary items. He may not be able
to make a proper choice of the lexical items; violate selectional and categorization
rules more often than normal children. (See Chomsky 1965 for an explication of
these types of rules).
Disorders of reading may be due to lack of proper instruction and opportunity,
poor attention, difficulty in perception, reduced vocabulary, and acquisition
of non-standard manner of using linguistic items. It may also be due to neurophysiological
reasons. Disorders of spelling may result from the disorder of reading. In writing
the child transfers the speech into a graphic form. For writing to be an effective
medium, it must be developed into a synthesis of movements and concepts. Lack
of such synthesis may be considered as a disorder along with the child's improper
hand movements, illegible handwriting, failure to produce coherent sentences in
writing and so on.
Here we will not present any of the techniques we have for proper therapy or for
simple training. Elucidation of such techniques and applications is beyond the
scope of the present work. In fact applications of linguistics have not been developed,
elucidated and explained with reference to their potential and rationale for use
in speech therapy by linguists. Linguistic applications are assumed generally
to be beyond the domains of linguists. We shall, however, point out how theories
of linguistics can influence work on speech disorders. Before taking it up, it
will not be out of place to report on Roman Jakobson's contributions to the analysis
of speech impairments.
Of all the linguists Roman Jakobson is the one and perhaps the only one who, during
his six decades of chequered linguistics career, has been consistently showing
interest in the studies of language disorders. He links these disorders with the
process of language acquisition and shows that there is a very intimate relationship
between problems of normal language in operation and the disintegration of language
as revealed by the different types of aphasic impairments. Because of this intimate
relationship Jakobson asserts that linguists have more than a marginal role to
play in the studies of language disorders, particularly in the study of aphasia.
He finds that aphasic impairments have their own order, a hierarchy of disorders.
It is the science of language that will be of immediate assistance in providing
tentative answers to the kinds of aphasia encountered in a particular case, as
the verbal disturbances do not present a unitary type but differ widely in quantitative
and qualitative scales among themselves.
There are two factors, namely selection and combination, which characterize a
normal language in operation. The selection of linguistic entities in normal language
is based upon the internal relationships that exists between the entities. These
relationships may be in terms of likeness, similitude, equivalence, resemblance,
analogy, diverse grades of specification and contrast. The combination is the
external relation of contiguity that exists between forms, relatable in terms
of neighbourhood, proximity and remoteness, subordination and coordination. The
aphasic disturbances also may be profitably looked at from the point of view of
these two factors.
Jakobson, in fact, identifies three dichotomies as underlying the cardinal types
of aphasia characterized by Luria and others. There are (i) combination which
implies continuity and affects primarily encodement versus selection, (ii) successivity
versus simultaneity and (iii) disintegration versus limitation. We shall see the
implication of these dichotomies when we present below the cardinal types of aphasic
impairments.
Drawing illustrations from the works and publications of Luria and others, Jakobson
builds his own linguistic interpretations of a aphasic impairments. He suggests
that Luria's basic classification of aphasic into six types of impairments holds
good even from the linguistic angle. Of these, two basic types clearly exemplify
the combination and selection dichotomy. The combination and selection dichotomy
is viewed also from the point of differences between encoding and decoding or
motor and sensory as done by other scholars. The six types of aphasic impairments
are - (i) efferent aphasia, (ii) sensory aphasia, (iii) amnesic aphasia.
In efferent
aphasia which is a basic type of encoding disorder words are preserved but the
patient has a lot of difficulty in the construction of sentences. These words
are generally independent of contexts and are mainly substantive concrete nouns.
Purely grammatical words disappear. These purely grammatical words include connectives
(conjunctions and pre-positions) as well as pronouns. The preservation and retention
of a word in this disorder is related directly to its independent status-whether
such a word can occur independently without purely grammatical props. Hence nouns
are preserved better than verbs. Only the nominative case survives the impairment.
Verbs are preserved in their most nominalized form. If a language has both an
infinitive form and a finite form of verb, it is the infinite form which shows
higher resistance in efferent aphasia. The speech of the aphasic is now more like
the telegraphic style. His utterances tend to be reduced tone word sentences.
In the phonological level, the phonemes are preserved, but the aphasic exhibits
difficulty in making the combinations of phonemes. He shows difficulty in the
transition from one phoneme to another. He makes an intensive use of phonemic
assimilation and dissimilation. 'The more independent a phoneme or distinctive
feature is in respect to the context, the greater the probability of its survival'.
The prosodic features are more affected than the inherent distinctive features
of phonemes, as the former involve interphonemic relations.
The efferent aphasia thus involves impairment of contiguity, and is a typical
contiguity disorder. This contiguity disorder is noticed on all levels of language.
The root is better preserved than the suffixes. The main deficiency relates to
the loss of ability to construct proposition through a construction of sentences.
If sentence construction is the major deficiency in the efferent aphasic, it is
the preservation of independent words that becomes the major deficiency in the
sensory aphasic. The sentence wholes are preserved, that is, the sentence pattern
is in tact. This is made possible because the grammatical words such as connectives,
and pronouns are preserved and such preservation provides the patterns easily.
To a sensory aphasic it is the initial substance that poses the greatest difficulty.
Hence in languages where the subject appears at the beginning of a sentence, the
predicate is more easily retained. The abolition of subject is more prominent
and more certain when the subject is a pure lexical items in its primary form
occurring with a minimum of dependence on the context. The aphasic will have greatest
difficulty in constructing an equational type of sentence. He will have difficulty
in naming objects.
In the phonological level, the sensory aphasic preserves the combinations of phonemes.
However, he simplifies some phonemes within the combinations. Such simplification
involves mainly those phonemes which cannot be predicted from their environment.
The major loss in the phonological level is the total loss of certain phonemic
distinctions. It is the phonemic perception and not the physical production that
characterizes the sensory aphasic. The phonologic defects of the sensory aphasic
are related to the hierarchical structure of the phonemic patterns. One can certainly
identify the order of deficits, their hierarchical order.
The two basic of aphasia, namely, the combination and selection are closely related
to encoding and decoding impairments. The combination disturbances hamper the
construction of a context, the encoding activity, as sentence construction is
seriously impaired. The selection disturbances hamper the analysis of a context
into its constituents, as sentence wholes are retained but a further breakdown
into constituent elements is not achieved.
Jakobson
believes that the constituents are intact in the encoding activity because the
speaker makes the selection of the elements before combining them into a whole.
In the decoding activity the decoder has to grasp the whole first and then proceeds
with the constituent analysis. 'The decoder is a probabilist to a much greater
extent than the encoder'. For the encoder the selection of items into a whole.
For the decoder, the grasping of the whole is the first stage which is followed
by an analysis of the constituents. Hence Jakobson finds that in both the basic
types of aphasia the first and primary stage is more viable and stable than the
second and secondary stage. Yet there is no pure encoding or decoding impairment.
The impairment is generally predominantly encoding or predominantly decoding.
Between decoding and encoding it is decoding that seems to occur more independently.
Decoding depends less upon encoding than vice versa. The recorded cases clearly
demonstrate the wider occurrence of decoding action than the encoding activity.
Agrammatism is the pivotal sign of efferent speech, external utterances are produced.
Impairment of internal speech is noticed in the efferent aphasia. The sensory
aphasia leads to an incapacity for metalingual operations which include paraphrasing,
use of synonyms and intralingual translation.
The third type of aphasia is called dynamic aphasia. The dynamic aphasic resembles
the efferent aphasic in many respects, because dynamic aphasia is also a combination
disorder. But the aphasic's difficulty is not with regard to combination of phonemes
or with regard to the combination of words in a sentence. The dynamic aphasic
fails to combine sentences which have no hierarchical grammatical rules within
a set of utterances. Thus the dynamic aphasic has a lot of difficulty in constructing
discourse and to build a monologue, 'a context which is incumbent on the speaker
alone'. He is unable to switch from one system of signs to another such as answering
a verbal order by a prescribed gesture.
Just as dynamic aphasia reflects a more intricate efferent aphasia, the semantic
aphasia reflects a higher order sensory aphasia. The semantic aphasic cannot grasp
the difference between phrases such as wife's brother and brother's wife. The
semantic aphasia reflects a highly attenuated form of sensory aphasia. The word
order is uniform and inflexible. The organization of a sentence pattern is easy
for the semantic aphasia. However, comprehension of a word is in direct proportion
to its syntactic environment. Morphology, in the words of Jakobson, yields to
syntax. Morphology of lexical items is completely lost on the semantic aphasia.
The afferent or kinesthetic aphasia is based on a disruption of the capacity for
combination and hence should be considered as an encoding disturbance. The afferent
aphasic however makes a merger of phonemes - not a mere assimilation. His failure
to combine appropriate features into a phoneme results in overlapping and merger.
He has difficulty in the combination of distinctive features into a phoneme. The
aphasic finds it difficult to use the bundle of all the concurrent features essential
for a phoneme and utilizes only one or a few features of the given phoneme which
by now shows a merger with one or more phonemes. There is no constancy however
is the repertory of preserved features and the terms of any binary opposition
are mutually inter-changeable. The afferent aphasia thus does not exhibit the
correspondence between the dichotomy of combination and selection, or between
encoding and decoding.
In the amnesic aphasia the patient is unable to make an appropriate selection.
But this inability is more particularly affecting iterative selection. The patient
points correctly to his eye when asked to do so. When asked to point out his eye
and ear, he points correctly to the first item and either omits the second or
shows a wrong organ. When asked to point out eye, ear and nose in a sequence he
is simply perplexed. In the grammatical level the coordinative constructions are
the only ones which suffer in the amnesia aphasia.
Jakobson's main contribution is the clarity with which he classified the aphasic
impairments purely on linguistic levels. The lingustic level envisaged by Jakobson
is an all comprehensive approach, not a mere structuralist position. The level
included phonological, morphological, syntactic, semantic and inter and intralingual
communication with their attendant discourse and paraphrasing capabilities. Jakobson's
contribution does not stop with a mere linguistic characterization of aphasic
impairments on purely linguistic levels, but to take it to a higher inter-disciplinary
interpretation.
Jakobson suggests two stages of analysis and pursuit for every scientific endeavour.
Autonomy of approach is an essential first step for a proper perspective and scientific
analysis of any phenomenon. At the same time autonomy is rewarding but isolation
is harmful. After an autonomous analysis of a phenomenon from the point of view
of a single discipline, it is useful and even necessary to look for correlation
of facts identified by different disciplines for the same phenomenon. The need
for such a correlation is much more urgent and fruitful for an investigation of
language disorders as these disorders have a definite but difficult to localize
bases in the neurophysiological make up of man.
Jakobson suggests a tentative topographic analogue to all the three linguistic
dichotomies identified by him :
Disorders
Topographic analogues
Combination Disorder
| More anterior
lesions of the cortex |
Selection
Disorder | More
posterior lesions of the cortex |
Efferent
Disorder | Anterotemporal
lesions |
Sensory
Disorder | Posterotemporal
lesions |
Dynamic
impairment | The
frontal intrinsic area of the forebrain |
Semantic
impairment | The
posterior intrinsic area (the posteroparietal and parieto-occipital sections).
|
The
Behaviourist-linguist and language disorders :
the behaviourist approach to language characterizes language as consisting of
a set of complex verbal responses. These responses are conditioned by environmental
reinforcement. There are two types of conditioning-classical and operant. In ameliorating
the conditions of speech disorders, the clinical procedure following the behaviourist
school insists upon repeated imitative tasks in which the rewards are awarded
for the correct responses. The pattern of responses is progressively shaped and
led to form more complex and more differentiating patterns. Sequencing of the
events for the imitative tasks, and the elicitations are carefully planned in
such a way that there is a slow but steady withdrawal of rewards with commensurate
increase in the non-imitative responses. Each step is considered a programme by
itself and the clinician proceeds from one step to the other only when the patient
has mastered the first step. The behaviourist may also use the developmental schedules
available for language in their clinical work.
In the behaviourist approach the emphasis is on the child's imitative learning.
If the child produces an utterance, comprehension is assumed to have been achieved.
Gradation of linguistic structures, in this approach encourages the identification
of nouns as the first step, followed by naming of objects and pictures, the locative
pre or post positions and so on. The aim is to enable the child to acquire the
basic elements of a language in all its levels, with the assumption that with
such an acquisition of basic elements the child will be in a position to develop
his language on his own initiative.
Language
acquisition is viewed as the learning of responses. These responses are reinforced
by rewards ; the rewards are given by a listener through comprehension, environmental
control, social approval and other actions. They reinforce the responses and the
responses become generalized linguistic rules. The reinforcement schedule includes
reinforcement of many items that are not, properly speaking, linguistic; but these
are highly essential non-linguistic props for the acquisition of language. These
include rewards for sitting still, contacting the eyes of the clinician and imitating
simple motor acts. Vocal imitation of vowels and consonants and repetition of
words and phrases immediately follow the above. On the receptive side, pointing
to objects, body parts and environmental conditions form the responses. Such responses
are also carefully graded. They are followed by the development of receptive responses
to simple commands. In the production side labeling plays an important role and
in the receptive side recognition has a prominent place. A useful and very crucial
linkage is presented to the child through games such as naming missing objects.
Storage and retrieval part of language capacity is aimed at in these exercises.
The when, where, how and why of things and events come later on, which aim at
making discourses in real language use situations.
The problem a clinician faces in a speech clinic is similar in many respects to
the one a second language teacher faces in his classroom. The patient is trained
in the clinic so that he will freely use this training outside. The problems of
performance outside the clinic thus must be reflected in the training programme
pursued in the clinic. Many a programme does not, however, recognize this fact.
Some however incorporate conversational and/or playgroup activities. Questions
and answers are presented as language tasks. The role of imitation is reduced
to some extent. In India programmes are few in number, publications of literature
are still a rarity, communication between scholars still carried out in terms
of work on languages of the west, and works based on Indian languages are yet
to be firmly established. Emphasis is on expression system rather than comprehension.
Yet language performance outside the clinic continues to receive only scant attention.
The
mentalist linguist and language disorders :
the mentalist linguist's approach to language disorders and their clinical amelioration
emphasizes the developmental schedule of language acquisition. He sequential amelioration
steps closely parallel the normal language acquisition developmental schedule.
Comprehension is considered basic and a pre-requisite to speech production. Normal
children have more receptive than productive power. Furthermore acquisition capacity
for comprehension is accomplished much earlier and is more elaborate than capacity
for production. As a corollary the relevance and importance of imitation should
be considered marginal. The performance of the patient outside the clinic has
a direct bearing on the ameliorative training administered in the clinic. The
mentalist records not only what the patients says, but also what the normal users
of the language say as well as the activity the patient is engaged in. Unlike
the behaviourist linguist-inspired clinician concentrates on syntactic and semantic
aspects along with phonological factors. The mentalist linguist-inspired clinician
assumes that grammatical analysis of early speech will help early diagnosis which
is distinct from language delay. This has resulted in the construction of several
developmental tests such as Berry-Talbott Exploratory test of Grammar, North Western
Syntax Screening Test and others. The mentalist clinician's major problem is the
interpretation and use of diverse and at times disparaging developments within
the mentalist school to his own advantage. The rapid changes that take place in
theory are a challenge to his nerve and skill. For some models and assumptions
of linguistics and learning theories, see Thirumalai (forthcoming).
To conclude, the aspects of language acquisition by normal children form a relevant
and very useful backdrop for the ameliorative steps. The various models of linguistics,
neurophysiology and learning theories should be carefully understood, properly
integrated and/or differentiated to achieve the best possible results.
N O T E S
The study of language, disorders and of the neurophysiological mechanisms of language
production, use and comprehension is yet to find its due place in the discipline
of linguistics. The students of linguistics and psycholinguistics will benefit
a lot from an understanding of the neurophysiological mechanisms and language
disorders. Linguists can contribute to scientific appraisal and evolution of ameliorative
steps badly needed to alleviate the suffering of speech patients. Brain (1961)
and Perkins (1971) give a good introduction to the types of speech disorders.
Luchsinger and Arnold (1965) may be also referred to. Penfield and Roberts (1959)
is another important volume. Jakobson (1971) gives aspects of aphasia. Berry (1969)
gives language disorders of children and suggests several ameliorative for an
indepth study of the positions of Luria and Lashley. Hebb's Organization of Behaviour.
(Hebb 1949) is a classic that must be read. For an understanding of speech disorders
and the practices of therapy, Berry and Eisenson (1970) is recommended. Reuck,
and O'connor (1964) is an excellent collection on disorders of language. Cherry
(1957) and Miller (1970) give aspects of the psychology of communication. For
auditory perception and acoustic perception of speech and communicative competence
we have already cited some references.