Literacy methodology
Reading & Spelling

Implications of Reading Research for Adult Literacy

Prev | Home | Next

ABSTRACT

An attempt has been made in this paper to outline principles that readers, skilled or beginners, may use to identify linguistic units and how these may be integrated with planning instruction in reading. We have attempted to isolate what readers seem to learn, how they refine strategies at the early stages of reading so that these may be used for instructional purposes. The features that distinguish written language at various levels have been identified and the abstract relations between written and spoken codes have been related to reading. The problem of how written information is divided into chunks of the use of letter sequences in the light of constraints on sound sequences has been considered. The question of integration of new and old skills has been raised and the discussion is concluded by outlining the training of efficient readers. Most of the theoretical points discussed need further testing and validating in Indian conditions.

In India we need to establish reading research in a fully co-ordinated manner since sporadic and unrelated studies do not present a coherent picture. What is needed is a reading research programme run by linguists, psychologists and educators to study acquisition of reading, reading skills, transfer from perceptional and analytic skills, reading speed, reading habits, goals and problems in reading etc. This is by no means an exhaustive list but only intended to indicate the wide ranging area of reading research. When one turns to current reading research (Gibson & Levin : 1975) from other countries to see what lines of development it projects, it appears that it has mainly concentrated on he school going child (6 + ) as the typical reader. Research with this age-group leads to the concurrent study of language acquisition as a maturational process and as a set of acquired skills. These children learnt to read in literate societies. Can this research have any implications for the problems of an adult learning to read ?

In Indian rural contexts we get literate as well as semi-literate societies. Are the learning strategies determined by age, by the amount of previous linguistic experience, or by transfer of skills from tasks which involve mental skills similar to those involved in reading ?

Studies that are concerned with adult learners have focused on the skilled reader. Although there are some studies of the would be reader, more attention has been paid to the accomplished reader. Experiments have measured the rate of reading, the degree of comprehension, the modes of organization of the meaning and the retention of content. There is also a great deal of interest in how new reading skills are integrated with old. (Wright, 1964 ; Kolers, 1964 ; Levin and Willliams, 1970 ; Smith, 1971).

From the perspective of this research adult educators may be able to see the relevance of the problem solving, how information derived from a written page is classified and the eventual integration of new knowledge with that already acquired.

An adult, who is learning to read, brings to the task of two kinds of well established strategies ; strategies for processing visual information and strategies for processing linguistic codes as far as the spoken language is concerned. In order to survive, the adult constantly determines which features of visual information are relevant. Similarly in problem solving, the adult lives. If adult educators wish to relate formal learning situations, then these situations must be observed and analysed. Learning to read involves discrimination of visual cues and therefore acquisition of reading is facilitated by the general strategy for problem solving which includes the sequence of collection of information and record keeping (Eisorfer and Lawton : 1973 ; Cole and Scribner : 1974) It has been pointed out that behaviour, which means using senses for oriented or observing, shares many functional properties with systematic search necessary to problem solving. Even in children, by the age of six or seven, which is the school going age, random trails give way to systematic search in problem solving-systematic search which is deliberate, sequentially organized and has internally consistant logic. This search involves formulating of a hypothesis and testing ; consequentially rejecting or accepting the hypothesis. A skilled reader goes through a similar process and his skill can be measured in terms of the number of correct allowable predictions he make about the still unread portion of the text as well as his earlier knowledge of the language.

Attention is a known prerequisite of any learning behaviour and it is equally important in selection, organization and interpretation of visual information. The child at six plus, learning to read, is selecting distinctive features of graphemes, organizing those in sets and interpreting visual information at different linguistic levels. Can children and adults be pretrained in selective attention so that reading is facilitated? It is not only possible but advisable to plan on pertaining in problem solving or selective observation. The situational context in such pretraining must be varied so that the skills trained can be transfered to discrimination of graphemes, associating of grapheme phoneme pairs and wholistic recognition of words and sentences or texts.It is interesting to note at thus point that children and adults who cannot read can discriminate between varities of written information, between prose and poetry, between bills and advertisements, between information works at higher levels of structure before it works at lower levels needs more careful study and discrimination. What is cited here are casual observations.

While the transfer of skills from observational behaviour is known, the problem of possible interference from irrelevant observing response has not as yet been investigated. In order to understand interference, cross model interference i.e., auditory as well as visual, it is necessary to look at basic psychological processes involved in reading which explain how selective attention is developed, how irrelevantstimulation is eliminated. That irrelevant visual stimulation does not interfere beyond a certain limit points to the existence of a filtering process i.e., that which separates for distinctive features and a unique pattern combination of these features. The other interesting question is about the nature of the scheme which the reader has focussed his attention on and with which he compares the stimulus pattern. This is the reader's search stratergy about which we do not know much.

The next question to be determined is the role of naming letters If letters are bundles of distinctive features, naming them sets them apart from other letters. If he discovery involves subvocal naming of letters, what kind of auditory background confusions will hamper this process especially when the medium of instruction is audio-visual ? The mediation of names of letters suggests further lines of inquiry and experimentation in methods of teaching reading.

Critical discrimination, it may be predicted, will be equally important in letter discrimination. Another thing we do not know about search strategy is whether inputs which share certain features are processed in a parallel fashion neisser or through a hierarchy of differentiating features. Further, we need also to find out whether age influences search strategy and if it does then we need to use different techniques in reading readiness programmes for children and adults.

Reading involves not only association of patterns of visual stimuli with aural-oral pattern but also retention of these associations and recalling these in new contexts. Skill in reading depends on the facility with which the reader learns to decode new visual sequences. on the establishing of transfer from known to unknown patterns, It can be predicted that such a transfer of skill is found only where the components of the code have been learnt. This would mean that reders taught by the whole word method and who as yet have not learnt component to sound correspondences, will not makes this transfer, However, it is possible to hypothesize that with adult learners it may not be necessary to teach component correspondence overtly and deliberately ;they would very probably the code themselves. This needs extensive testing.

The role of higher order units in facilitating perception and retention of written language has been known for some time now, but is has had no impact on the teaching of reading. While there is evidence to show that syntactic and spelling patterns influence the perception of written language, it is not clear what role the sequential probabilities of one letter following another play in the perception of a word. Beyond the letter to letter probabilities, are there any rules for mapping sequences of letters to sound which might help in forming units of reading ?

In his book on "Linguistics and Reading" Fries explored he role of spelling patterns in orthography, Hocket and venezky worked out for spelling to sound mapping. A hypothesis derived from this work that spelling sound correspondences facilitate unit formation was tested in tachitoscopic experiments in the Cornell Laboratory and the finding was that pronounceable combinations are read better than unpronounceable ones. This is evident in the response of young subjects at the age of nine plus. Surprisingly enough even congenitally deaf subjects (College students) read pronounceable combinations with greater ease. This indicates that spelling patterns have a structure of their own. Sound is not necessary for processing higher order units in reading. A skilled reader uses his knowledge of such rules and of the structure of his language and higher order relation between systems. A skilled reader may process written material at different levels probably simultaneously and in so doing use graphic information processed into chunks ; and how he does this needs to be studied. How does a non-accomplished reader, child or adult, discover rules for himself in mapping sequences of letters to sound which process graphic material into units for reading ? This has been explored by Gibson et al (1967). They tried to find out whether when a beginning reader (5 + ) learns to discriminate members of a set, he makes use of the structural constraints within he stimulus set and then does he learn to look for such structure in new problems consisting of similar kind of material. Further, does this interself faciliate abstraction of patterns where the structure involved is different ? The finding showed evidence of forming learning sets progressive increase in problems solved correctly. This should be of great interest to adult educators in planning their work and such findings need to be duplicated with adult learners.

For a long time now reading research has tried to integrate structural discoveries of linguistic science with reading instruction in an attempt to improve it. Great deal of thought has been given to how young readers use their knowledge of spoken language in learning to read. More recent developments in the formal analysis of syntax and semantics could also be used to understand different aspects of reading skills. There can be two ways in which grammar can be used in the acquisition of reading skill. (Bever and Bower, 1966). While great many readers process visual inputs directly without recourse to the mediation of auditory process. It can be hypothesized hat 'Visual readers' can comprehend written language faster and better than 'auditory readers'. This implies that reading can and should be taught as a visual skill enabling the reader to analyse written sentences into their fundamental psychological dieectly structure, with-out auditory intervention. If a reader can be trained to arrive at linguistically correct ans pertinent syntactic and semantic interpretation from the way written sentences look rather than form the way they might sound than dependence on habits learnt in auditory perception call be avoided.

Most transformational linguists today will accept that the perception and comprehend of a sentence involves the discovery of an appropriate deep structure for which the semantic rules can provide an interpretation and that the phonological and the surface phrase structure is more directly representated in speech but the deep structure is not represented directly. However, many experiments indicate that the discovery of the underlying structure is the perceptional goal of the listener or the reader. REaserch has shown that subjects use the underlying structure of sentences to form perceptual sets, and to remember sentences. Skill in reading m,ay then be viewed as an ability to exract from the visual information the undeeerlying structure of sentences. Of course, any reader, child or adult, has a well developed perceptual routing for analysis of spoken language and he can choose to use auditory perception routine in reading. However the reader also has well established routines in processing visual information and therefore nothing can interfere if he wants to develop an entirely visual perceptual strategy for the comprehension of written sentences. Readers using this independent strategy would develop underlying structures directly from visual inputs. In both non-visual and visual reading there is maximization of the use of available psychological processes. Non-visual or auditory readers may be limited in the speed and accuracy with which they read, comprehend and remember, since there is a normal speed at which spoken language proceeds. Several studies indicate that while an auditory reader gets superficial details of sentences correctly, the visual reader is less concerned with the phonological and actual surface structure of the sentences and more with the underlying structure. Another finding is that while 'auditory readers' tend to process material from left to right to left depending on the way their language is written, 'visual readers' are free to process written material in any order that facilitates direct visual discovery or underlying structure.

It appears that the first word recognized by 'visual' readers was the underlying subject regardless of the phrase relation in the phrase structure. Since visual readers can comprehend faster and better than non-visual readers and are unable to identify the syntactic form of the sentences, it is necessary to test these findings at levels higher than the sentence and then study the methods by which a visual reader may be trained. It has been found that the attentional process underlying visual reading is docile and easily trained. Therefore, a visual reader is not only more efficient but it is easy to learn to become a visual reader. The significance of this to reading programmes in adult education is obvious.


R E F E R E N C E S

Basso, K. H. "The ethnography of writing." In R. Bauman and J. Sherzer (Eds).
Explorations in the ethnography of speaking. London : Cambridge University Press, 1975. pp. 425-432.

Cipolla, C. M. Literacy and development in the West. Baltimore : Penguin Books, 1969.

Cole, M and Scribner, S. Culture and though. New York : Wiley, 1974.

Eisdorfer, D. and Lawton, M. P. (eds.). The psychology of adults developments and
aging. Washington : American Psychological Association, 1973.

Ferguson, C. "DIglossia". Word, 15, 1959 325-340. (Reprinted in D. Hymes (Ed.).
Langauge in culture and society. New York : Harper and Row, 1964, pp.
429-439).

Ferguson, C. "Language development". In J. Fishman, C. Perguson and J. Das Gupta
(Eds.), Language problems of development nations. New York : Wiley, 1968, pp.
27-35.

Foder, J. A. Bever, T. G. and Garrett, M. F. The Psychology of language, New York :
McGraw Hill, 1974.

Friedrich, P. "Anthropological linguistics ; recent research and immediate prospects."
Georgetown University Monograph Series on Language and Linguistics, 24, 1971, 167-184.

Gibson, E.J. and Levin, H. The psychology of reading. Cambridge, Mass. : M I T Press,
1975.

Goddy, J. (Ed.) Literacy in traditional societies. Cambridge University Press, 1965.

Grimes, J. The thread of discourse. The Hague : Mouton, 1975.

Gudschinsky, S. C. A manual of literacy for preliterate peoples. Ukarumpa, Papua New
Guinea : Summer Institute of Linguistics, 1973.

Harman, D. Community fundamental education, Lexington, Mass, ; D. C. Heat, 1974.

Hymes, D. Foundations of sociolinguistics. Philadelphia : University of Pennsylvania
Press, 1974.

Jones, R. F. The triumph of the English language. Stanford, Calif, : Stanford University
Press, 1953.

Kaplan, R. The anatomy of rhetoric. Philadelphia : Centre for Curriculum Development,
1972.

Kavanagh, J. and Mattingly, I. Reading by ear and by eye. Cambridge Mass.: M I T
Press, 1972.

Labov. W. Sociolinguistic patterns. Philadelphia : U n I v e r s I t y of Pennsylvania
Press, 1972a.

Lambert, W. and Gardner, R. Attitudes and motivation in second language learning.
Rowley, Mass. : Newbury House, 1972.

Levin, H. and Williams, J. P. Basic studies on reading. New York Basic Books, 1970.

Levin, H. (ed.) Project Literacy reports 1-5, 1964-67 Ithaca. New York.

Mattingly, I, "Reading, the linguistic process, and linguistic awareness". In J. F.
Kavanagh and I. G. Mattingly (eds.)., Language by ear and by eye. Cambridge,
Mass.: M I T Press, 1972, pp. 133-148.

O' Neil, W. "The spelling and pronunciation of English." In W. Morris (Ed.) The
American Heritage Dictionary of the English language. New York : American Heritage Publishing, 1969. pp. xxxv-xxxvii.

Ruiz, R. Mexico : The challenge of Poverty and illiteracy. San Maricon, Calif.:
Huntingon Library, 1963.

Smith. F. Understanding reading. New York : Holt, Rinehart, Winston, 1971.

Torrey, J. "Learning to read without a teacher." Elementary Englidh. 46, 1969, 550-556,
658.

Vachek, J. Written language : general problems and problems of English. The Hague :
Mouton, 1973.

Venezky, R. The structure of English orthography. The Hague : Mouton, 1970.

Wardhaugh, R. Reading : a linguistic perspective. New York : Harcourt Brace, 1969.


ADDITIONAL REFERENCES

Kolers, Paul A. "Reading as a perceptual skill" in Project Literacy Reports No. 1.
Cornell University, Ithaca. New York 1964.

Wrihght, John C. "Acquired Relevance of Cues in Reading" in Project Literacy Reports
No. 1. Cornell University. Ithaca, New York, 1964.

Gibson, ELenor, J. Forrber James and Shepela Sharon. "Test of a Learning Set Procedure
for the Abstraction of Spelling Patterns" in Project Literacy Reports No. 8. Cornell University, Ithaca, New York, 1967.

Fries, C. Linguistics and Reading.

Bever, Thomas and Bower, Thomas. "How to Read without Listening" in Project
Literacy Reports No. 6. Cornell University, Ithaca, New York 1966.

Neisser, U. "Visual Search." In Scientific American, 210, 1964, 94-102.