Chapter - 1 PHONOTACTICS
&
MORPHOPHONEMICS
Tangkhul-Naga
Grammar ( A Study of Word Formation )
Prev
| Home | Next 1.0 Introduction There
are 7 vowels and 21 consonants in Tangkhul-Naga.
As suprasegmental features, there are tones, length and nasality. The vowels are nasalized in the vicinity of
nasals consonants. Inter-nasal vowels
are vowels are always nasalized while pre-nasals or post-nasals are slightly nasalized.
Nasalization of vowels, therefore, is not phonemic and the nasal vowels
are the contextually conditioned variants of
the oral ones. Also, there is a large number of freely varying varieties
of vowels and vowel clusters conditioned by different pitch heights and intonations. 1.1
Phonemic
inventory of Tangkhul-Naga Schematically
we can present the phonemic inventory of the language as follows : Vowels :
Consonants
:
| | Bila-bial | Labio-dental | Dental-
alveolar | Palatal | Velar | Glo-ttal |
O
b s t r u e n t s | Plosive | p ph | | t th | c | k kh | ? |
Fricative | | F | z s | | | h |
Nasal
| m | | n | | η | |
S
o n o r a n t s | Lateral | | | l | | | |
Thrill | | | r | | | |
Approximant | w | v | | Y | | |
Suprasegmentals
:
(i) |
Length |
= |
: |
|
(ii) | Nasality | = | ~ | |
(iii) | Tones: | High | = | ΄e.g.
phá = rt. of ‘good’) |
| | Mid | = | Not
marked (e.g. pha = rt. of ‘search’) |
| | Low | = | (e.g.
phà = rt. of ‘pluck’) |
1.2 Allophonic
distribution of vowels In all there are 11 vowel sounds in Tangkhul-Naga. All the vowels except [u], [o] and [∂] have allophones. [i], [e] , [a] and [ü, ū] respectively.
In the following discussion, however, we have to note that the difference
between tense and lax pairs such as [i]
and [I], [e]
and [ε],[a]and[ā] and [ü, ū] respectively. In the following discussion, however, we have
to note that the difference between tense and lax pairs such as [i] and [I], [e] and [ε,[a]and[ā] is not very significant in the
sense that they are in free variation and their differences are not predictable
in terms of their position in a word. Comparatively,
the difference between the allophones [ū] and [ü] is easily predictable in terms of their position
in a word. For the rest vowel phonemes
and allophones the following examples show only the ‘more acceptable’ pronunciation.
(The allophonic variations will be indicated in the section only). We can present all the vowel sounds as under:
[i] has two allophones - - [i] and [I]. They occur freely except
in word initial position where [i] is
conspicuous by its absence. [i], the high unrounded front vowel, occurs in word medial and final
positions. It occurs with all tones.
Medially |
Finally |
thiη | ‘wood’ | à-th í | ‘gall bladder’ |
sìna | ‘gold’ | hi | ‘boat’ |
tín | ‘spade’ | m∂̀í | ‘salt’ |
[I], the lower high unrounded
front vowel, occurs initially, medially and finally and with all tones.
Initially |
Medially |
Finally |
I | ‘I’ | niη | ‘mind’ | àšI | ‘blood' |
Ìk-r ü | ‘dew’ | šIm | ‘house’ | mÌ | ‘man’ |
Íyáowó | ‘alas!’ | mÌk | ‘eye’ | hI | ‘this’ | [e] has two allophones - - [e] and [ε]. [e],
the higher mid unrounded front vowel, occur initially, medially and finally
Initially |
Medially |
Finally |
éin∂ | ‘and,
with’ | šèm | ‘to make’ | me | ‘goat’ |
é? | ‘interjection
(for surprise)’ | nem | ‘be
low’ | k∂̀phe | ‘to
slap’ | [ε],
the mid unrounded front vowel occurs word finally after [r] and [l].
It occurs in low tone. Finally
Initially |
Medially |
v∂rè | ‘God’ | m ∂̀
lE$ | ‘ tongue’ |
kh∂̀
-rE$ | ‘first
, former’ | | | [a] has two allophones - - [a]
[A#] [a],
the low unrounded front vowel occurs initially, medially and finally and with
all tones.
Initially |
Medially |
Finally |
á-cü | ‘today’ | pàη | ‘hand’ | hà | ‘tooth’ |
à-rá | ‘style’ | yaη | ‘strength’ | sà | ‘meat’ |
à | ‘he/she’ | vár | ‘mushroom’ | kh∂̀nà | ‘ear’ | [A#], the higher low unrounded central
vowel, is slightly higher than [a]. It occurs only in word initial position with mid and low tones. Initially
Initially |
Medially |
À# - wón | ‘flower’ | À# -khón | ‘sound’ |
À# - ton | ‘top’ | À#-t ∂́m | ‘time’ | [∂], the mean mid
unrounded central vowel has no allophonic variations. It occurs medially and finally.
Initially occurs only with expressive words.
Initially |
Medially |
Finally |
∂̀̀i | ‘yes (I will) | z∂m | ‘liquor’ | m∂̀ | ‘yes (that’s right)’ |
∂s | ‘expression of disgust’ | v ∂̀t | ‘cotton’ | á-kh ∂́m∂ | ‘tomorrow’ | [o], the higher mid rounded back vowel has no allophonic variations.
It occurs initially, medially and finally and with all tones.
Initially |
Medially |
Finally |
ot | ‘thing’ | hòk | ‘swine’ | à-wò | ‘grandfather’ |
oca | ‘teacher’ | sop | ‘basket’ | šo | ‘lock |
oko | ‘box’ | koη | ‘river’ | po | ‘to
beg, request’ | [u], the high rounded back vowel has no
allophonic variation. It occurs initially,
medially and finally. It occurs with all
tones.
Initially |
Medially |
Finally |
ùk | ‘abdomen’ | lùk | ‘basket’ | m ∂cu | ‘color’ |
úrüwok | ‘spring,
well’ | tùì | ‘language/speech’ | -lù | ‘imperative suffix’ |
ùt | ‘camel’ | pùη | ‘hour/watch’ | hu | ‘poison’ | [ü], the high unrounded central vowel has two allophones - - [ü,
ū] [ü], occurs medially and finally. It occurs in the syllable final position
preceded by any consonant except the semi-vowel [w].
Medially |
Finally |
sǘkéí | ‘domestic animals’ | à-mü | ‘picture’ |
kǘla | ‘luck/shadow’ | à-rü | ‘liquid’ |
rümok | ‘name of a bird | násü | ‘dove’ |
[ū], the high unrounded back vowel occurs only finally preceded
by labio-dental fricative [f] and labio-dental
preceded by labio-dental fricative [f] and labio-dental approximant [v]. It occurs will all tones.
Finally |
f ū | ‘dog’ | à-vū́ | ‘mother’ |
m ∂̀f ū | ‘elephant’ | c ∂́mv ū | ‘cicada’ | 1.3 Vowel
sequences / diphthongs There
are seven types of diphthong in Tangkhul-Naga . They usually occur in syllable
final position. Initial vowel sequence is found only in expressive
word and some affixes. Diphthongs. Occurrence
Medially |
Finally |
ei | sèìhá | ‘prayer’ | mei | ‘fire’ |
| keinú η | ‘city’ | kh∂leì | ‘squirrel’ |
eo | réósa | ‘name
of a children’s game’ | k∂méó | ‘god,
demon’ |
| | | teo | ‘small’ |
∂u | th ∂una | ‘courage’ | th ∂u | ‘concern’ |
| c∂uki | ‘chair’ | c∂ú: | ‘yelling
sound in hunting’ |
ai | láírik | ‘book; | kháí | ‘fish’ |
| ráíci | ‘scabies’ | mai | ‘face’ |
ao | karkàò | ‘spider’ | yàò | ‘sheep’ |
| nàòmei | ‘gun’ | páó | ‘news’ |
oi | ---- | | šoi | ‘signature’ |
| ---- | | šòì | ‘to
fail’ |
ui | mùìya | ‘cloud’ | kh ∂̀
mùì | ‘bread’ |
| kúírü | ‘mister,
sir’ | kúí | ‘head’ |
1.4 Allophonic status of consonants In
all there are 21 consonant phonemes I Tangkhul- Naga.
Stops (p, t, k) have two allophones each - - [p,b], [t,d] and [k,g] respectively. Affricate [c] has two allophones - [c] and [j]. Aspirated stop [ph, th, kh],
glottal stop [?]. fricatives [f, z, s, š, h], nasal [n, η] and lateral [l] have no allophonic
variations. Bilabial nasal [m] and
trill [r] have two allophones each –
[m, M] and [r, r==] respectively. Approximants [v], [w] and [y] have no allophonic variation. The
opposition of voice and voicelessness is attested to fricatives only.
Elsewhere, voice is not a phonemic feature.
Voiced and voiceless plosives are in free
variation, and consequently the difference between [p] and [b], [t] and [d], or [k] and [g] are not
predictable in terms o f their position in a word. Thrill [r]
and [r==] are also in free variation.
Consequent6ly there is no distinction between :
mìk |
~ |
mìg |
‘eye’ |
|
kúí |
~ |
gúí |
‘head’ |
n∂p | ~ | n∂b | ‘mucus’ | | pà η | ~ | bà η | ‘hand’ |
zat | ~ | zad | ‘food’ | | tùì | ~ | dùì | ‘language’ |
oca | ~ | oja | ‘teacher’ | | ca | ~ | ja | ‘tea’ |
rǜrèì | ~ | rǜrèì | ‘python’ | | à-rü | ~ | à-rü | ‘liquid’ |
Studies in both
inter-group and intra-group variations shows that the allophones, except [m] and
[M],
are used in free variation. Hence, in
spite of the widely used, rather strictly,
orthography, I do not see the need to account [b, d, g, j, r=] as structured allophones of [p, t, k, c, r] respectively. Again, only a few educated speakers tend to
distinguish between [s] and [š]. One reason for the widespread occurrence
of free variation of the allophones may be that speakers of standardized Tangkhul-Naga
belong to some 20 villages having different dialects, and there is no conformity
in the majority speakers’ pronunciation. In some cases the British missionaries (who
invented the Tangkhul Naga orthography) might have arbitrarily invented some sounds
as structured phoneme of the language. For
instance, slightly nasalized voiceless alveolar fricative [š] is often mistaken with [S] which is
not found in any of the various village dialects, and we can readily conclude
that [S] is a post-British sound (tat is, invented by the missionaries). 1.5
Phonemic contrast and positional distribution
of consonants Distributionally,
almost all the consonant phonemes can occur in word initial and medial positions.
Aspirated stops [ph, th, kh],
glottal stop [?], affricate [c] and fricatives [f, s, z, š, h] am approximant
[v] do not occur in word final position. Now,
let us examine the distribution and phonemic contrast of consonants from the following
data. To illustrate the contrast, words
with the same tone have been selected as far as possible. [p] :
[ph]. Voiceless bilabial stop [p] occurs in word initial, medial and final positions. Voiceless aspirated bilabial stop [ph] occurs initially and medially.
|
[p] |
|
ph |
|
Initially
: | pùn | ‘hour,
watch’ | phuη | ‘drum’ |
| pà η | ‘hand’ | phà η | ‘a
poisonous tree’ |
Medially: | k∂̀pá | ‘bamboo’ | n∂ phò η | ‘to
float’ |
| k∂̀pei | ‘spoon’ | rü phei | ‘northern
Tangkul’ |
Finally
: | n∂p | ‘mucus’ | - - - - | |
| à-k∂́p | ‘machine’ | - - - - | | [t] : [th] Voiceless dental-alveolar
stop [t] occurs word initially, medially
and finally. Voiceless aspirated dental
alveolar stop [th] occurs initially and medially.
|
[t] |
|
[ph] |
|
Initial
: | tao | ‘luck’ | tháó | ‘oil’ |
| tí η | ‘sound
of bell’ | thiη | ‘wood’ |
Medially: | à-η∂̀tiη | ‘marrow’ | n∂̀thòr | ‘morning’ |
| η∂̀téí | ‘different’ | m∂̀thá | ‘good/nice’ |
Finally
: | zat | ‘food’ | - - - - | |
| v∂̀t | ‘cotton’ | - - - - | | [k]
: [kh] Voiceless velar stop [k] occurs word initially, medially and finally.
Voiceless aspirated velar stop [kh] occurs initially and medially.
|
[k] |
|
[kh] |
|
Initial
: | kúí | ‘luck’ | khùì | ‘oil’ |
| kán | ‘sound
of bell’ | kháη | ‘wood’ |
Medially: | m∂̀kéí | ‘marrow’ | š∂̀ηkhü | ‘morning’ |
| η∂̀káí | ‘different’ | rìkhan | ‘good/nice’ |
Finally
: | hòk | ‘food’ | - - - - | |
| mìk | ‘cotton’ | - - - - | | [f] : [v] Voiceless labio-dental
fricative [f] and voiced labio-dental
approximant /v/ occur in word initial and final positions. [f] occurs only if followed by high unrounded
central vowel [ü].
|
[s] |
|
[v] |
|
Initial
: | fü | ‘dog’ | vü | ‘feminine
marker’ |
| füηùm | ‘a
kind of wood pigeon’ | vànao | ‘bird’ |
Medially: | c∂̀rfü | ‘window’ | à-z∂rvü | ‘younger
sister of a man’ |
| r∂m̀fü | ‘fox’ | z∂̀mvü | ‘cicada’ |
[s] : [š] Both voiceless dental-alveolar fricative [s] and slightly nasalized voiceless dental
alveolar fricative [š] occur in word initial and medial positions. [s] occurs finally in some
expressives :
|
[f] |
|
[š] |
|
Initial
: | síráó | ‘antelope’ | šìràò | ‘a
species of dry land paddy’ |
| so | ‘to
bless’ | šó | ‘lock’ |
Medial: | η∂̀só | ‘to
be with’ | η∂̀š∂̀n | ‘custom’ |
| ŋ∂̀sǜ | ‘to
wash cloth’ | k∂̀šùì | ‘bobby
trap’ |
Final | ∂̀ís | ‘No!
I don’t think ….. | -
- - - | | [c] : [z] Voiceless palatal affricate [c] occurs initially and medially. Voiced dental-alveolar fricative [z] occurs only in word initial positions.
|
[c] |
|
[z] |
|
Initial
: | cat | ‘people,
tribe’ | zat | ‘food’ |
| cà | ‘necklace’ | zà | ‘to
eat’ |
Medial: | m∂̀cí | ‘salt’ | - - - - | |
| à-
η∂̀caη | ‘wing’ | -
- - - | | [h] : [?] Voiceless glottal fricative [h] occurs in
word initial and medial positions. Glottal
stop [?] occurs finally in some interjective and expressive words.
|
[h] |
|
[?] |
|
Initial
: | hui | ‘ginger’ | - - - - | |
| h∂m | ‘pot’ | - - - - | |
Medial: | à-húí | ‘skin’ | - - - - | |
| k∂̀hà | ‘bamboo’ | - - - - | |
Final | ∂̀ís | -
- - - | yo? | ‘expression
for surprise’ |
| | -
- - - | sà | ‘Do!
(imperative/urgency)’ | [m] : [M] Labio-dental
[M] is an allophone of bilabial [m]. [m] occurs in word initial, medial
and final positions. [M] occurs medially
followed by [v] or [f].
|
[m] |
|
[M] |
|
Initial
: | mì | ‘man’ | - - - - | |
| mei | ‘fire’ | - - - - | |
Medial: | à-m∂ n | ‘price’ | r∂̀Mfü | ‘fox’ |
| kh∂̀mà | ‘wound’ | c∂̀Mvü | ‘cicada’ |
Final | à-t∂ m | ‘time’ | -
- - - | |
| à-p∂ m | ‘place’ | -
- - - | | [n] : [η] Both dental-alveolar nasal [n]
and velar nasal [η] occur
in word initial, medial and final positions.
|
[n] |
|
[η] |
|
Initial
: | n∂ | ‘you’ | η ∂̀thúm | ‘hide’ |
| nàò | ‘child’ | ηàò | ‘expression
used in frightening children’ |
Medial: | η∂̀n∂̀m | ‘smell’ | kh∂ ηáí | ‘meadow’ |
| sìna | ‘gold’ | sì ηa | ‘back,
future’ |
Final | ròn | ‘to
lock’ | rò
η | ‘to
flow’ |
| tòn | ‘to
be awkward’ | tò
η | ‘to
ride’ | [l]
: [r]. Alveolar lateral [l] occurs in word initial
and medial positions. Alveolar trill [r]
occurs initially, medially and finally.
|
[l] |
|
[r] |
|
Initial
: | l∂m | ‘fathom’ | r∂̀m | ‘land’ |
| | | | |
Medial: | η∂̀lei | ‘land’ | η∂̀rei | ‘to
turn around’ |
| | | | |
Final | ---- | | zúr | ‘rainy
season’ |
| | | | | 1.6
Consonant clusters Only
a few consonant clusters (intrasyllabic) and a fairly large number of consonant
sequences (intersyllabic) are found in Tangkhul-Naga. Like other Tibeto-Burmic-Naga languages, the Tangkhul-Naga village dialects
are very rich in word/syllable initial consonant clusters. But this feature gradually got lost in today’s
standardized Tangkhul Naga with the introduction of school text books, dictionaries
and grammars, and religious books, which were all written and compiled by the
British missionaries in the late 19th century. Today’s speakers have acquired the form of
the non-native speakers’ works in which a lot of ‘vowel insertion’ had been made.
Now, there are less than 10 intrasyllabic consonant clusters in the language
which occur both in word initial and medial positions.
As a rule consonant cluster does not occur in syllable final positions.
As a rule consonant cluster does not occur in syllable final positions. 1.6.1. Initial cluster There
are nine types of two-member initial clusters
p + r |
préì-nao |
‘married woman’ |
ph
+r | phraηci | ‘a red woolen blanket’ |
η
+ k | ηk∂̀ì | ‘expression of unwillingness, rejection;
‘No’’ |
t
+ r | tràkhà-
thei | ‘grapes’ |
k
+ w | kwa-
thei | ‘pan (betal nut)’ |
k + r | kruη | ‘thundering sound’ |
kh + r | khrus | ‘cross’ |
th + r | thr∂ k | ‘sound of clearing phlegm-filled throat’ |
y + r | yhèr yhèr | ‘the way water flows out smoothly from
a pipe’ | 1.6.2. Medial clusters Almost
any pair of consonants can occur in word medial position in a sequence as a syllable
closing consonant, i.e. as coda margin, and as a syllable opening consonant, i.e.,
as an onset margin of a succeeding syllable. In many instances the clusters occur
across the morpheme boundaries of the word concerned.
Germinated consonants of the type – C1C1 – and intrasyllabic
medial clusters are very few in number. Within the word boundary, the following patterns are usually attested.
[Hyphen indicates syllable boundary] 1.6.2.1.
Three consonant clusters In
a medial three- consonant cluster, the first phoneme is either a nasal, a stop
of a trill; the second is a stop and the third is always trill [r].
mpr |
c∂m-pra |
‘lemon’ |
tthr | pìt-thrai | ‘bronze’ |
ηkr | khòη-krao | ‘trouzer’ |
kpr | prik-prǜ | ‘a
kind of spotted green pumpkin’ | 1.6.2.2.
Heterogeneous clusters All
the following examples of consonant clusters are of the inter-syllabic type, that
is, the first member of the sequence occurs a syllable closing consonant and the
second as a syllable opening consonant.
(i) |
Stop
+ Stop |
| p
+ t | h∂̀p-ta | ‘week’ |
| t
+ p | yòt-pi | ‘nail’ |
| k
+ kh | rìk-khan | ‘lightning’ |
| k
+ t | kok-tùì | ‘cuckoo’ |
(ii) |
Stop
+ Affricate/Fricative |
| t
+ c | hùt-ci | ‘parrot’ |
| t
+ s | l∂̀t-sai | ‘a
long-beaked ant-eating bird’ |
| k
+ c | kok-ca | ‘cicada’ |
| k
+ z | kàη-zei | ‘football’ |
(ii) | Stop
+ Nasal/Lateral/Trill |
| t
+ c | hùt-ci | ‘parrot’ |
| t
+ s | l∂̀t-sai | ‘a
long-beaked ant-eating bird’ |
| k
+ c | kok-ca | ‘cicada’ |
| k
+ z | Kàη-zei | ‘football’ |
(iii) |
Stop
+ Nasal/Lateral/Trill |
| p
+ l | lep-lei | ‘auger’ |
| t
+ n | hòt-nà | ‘to
try’ |
| t
+ l | hòt-là | ‘ash’ |
| t
+ r | ot-r∂ m | ‘service’ |
| k
+ l | lìk-li | ‘glass,
bottle’ |
| k+
r | rìk-rǘ | ‘porcupine’ |
(iv) |
Nasal
+ Stop |
| m
+ p | t∂ m-pak | ‘valley’ |
| m
+ t | l∂m-ta | ‘direction’ |
| n
+ t | kan-ta | ‘bell’ |
| n
+ th | kan-thá | ‘quilt’ |
| η
+ t | pu
η-ton | ‘guava’ |
| η
+ kh | h∂̀
η-kha | ‘crow’ |
(v) |
Nasal
+ Affrocate/Fricative |
| m
+ s | kham
- súí | ‘walking
stick’ |
| m
+ z | hàm-zik | ‘adder’ |
| m= + f | r∂̀m-fü | ‘jackal’ |
| M + v | c∂ m-vü | ‘cicada’ |
| n
+ c | l∂ n-ci | ‘straw’ |
| η
+ h | ph∂ η-hai | ‘husk’ |
| η
+ v | càη-véí | ‘shield’ |
| η
+ z | šàη-ziη | ‘name
of a village’ |
| n
+ v | z∂ n-và | ‘a
pretty-colored bird frequenting rocky spots’ |
(vi) |
Nasal
+ Lateral/Trill/Semi-vowel |
| m
+ l | kom-la | ‘orange’ |
| n
+ l | t∂ n-lùì | ‘name
of a village’ |
| n + r | c∂ n-rèì | ‘highway’ |
| n + y | zin-yór | ‘name
of a species of sesame’ |
| η
+ 1 | m∂
η-là | ‘soul’ |
| η
+ y | yà
η-yáó | ‘swallow’ |
| η
+ r | cà
η-réí | ‘name
of a species of paddy’ |
(vi) |
Trill
+ Non-trill |
| r
+ ph | khor-phù
η-là | ‘water
snail’ |
| r
+ t | yàr-tui | ‘shoulder’ |
| r + k | kar-kàò | ‘spider’ |
| r + f | c∂̀r-fü | ‘window’ |
| r
+ v | s∂̀r-và | ‘village
priest’ | From
the above data of medial consonant clusters in Tangkhul-Naga it may be observed
that the first members are nasals, lateral, trill and stops.
Aspirated stops, fricatives and affricates do not occur in syllable final
position in the language, and as such they do not occur as the first member in
the medial clusters. It is also to be
noted that all medial clusters shown above are inter-syllabic, and further combinations
are possible across the morpheme boundaries in various derived/inflected words. 1.7
Tone Tangkhul-Naga
is a tone language having lexically significant, contrastive, but relative pitch
on each syllable. Tone serves to distinguish
the meanings of words otherwise composed of the same sound. The contrastive, lexical units of sound are
phonemes, or, in tonal analysis, tonemes. There
are three ‘level’ tonemes and two ‘gliding’ tonemes in the language.
By definition, a level toneme is one in which the pitch of a syllable does
not rise or fall during its production whereas a gliding toneme is one in which
the pitch of a syllable rises or falls, or some combination of rise and fall such
as rising-falling or falling-rising occurs.
We
can represent the level tonemes and gliding tonemes as under :
|
Tone |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Level |
|
Gliding |
|
|
|
|
|
High |
= |
á |
High |
+ |
Mid |
|
|
= |
á |
Mid |
= |
a (not marked)
|
High |
+ |
Low |
|
|
= |
â |
Low |
= |
à |
Low |
+ |
Mid |
|
|
= |
a |
|
|
|
Low
|
+
|
High |
|
|
|
ǎ |
|
|
|
High |
+ |
Low |
+
|
High |
= |
ã |
|
|
|
Low |
+ |
High |
+
|
Low |
= |
a |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Level
tones : |
|
|
Mid | pha | ‘to
search’ |
High | phá | ‘be
good’ |
Low | phà | ‘to
pluck’ |
Mid | pai | ‘edible
bulb’ |
High | páí | ‘excreta’ |
Low | pàì | ‘to
fly’ |
Gliding
tones : |
|
|
Rising
-Falling | ηâ? | ‘buffaloes
crying sound (especially when they are about to fight)’ |
Falling-rising | ηǎ
: | ‘small
baby’s crying sound’ |
1.7.1.
Allotones Tangkhul-Naga tonemic rules are rather very regular. In most cases neighboring tonemes do not affect
one another. There may be a change in
the tone of a syllable in derivation and reduplication but not in compounding
and inflection. Changes may, of course,
occur to indicate interrogation emphasis, or the status of the speaker as compared
to that of the addressee, or it may reflects various implications, such as politeness,
threat, urgency, intensity, or proximity. The
change of tonemes may also be caused by factors like emotion, or stylistics.
Examples
: |
|
|
sei | (Mid
- toned) | ‘Let’s
go’ (request without respect) |
séi | (High
+ Mid) | ‘Let’
go’ (request without respect) |
seì | (Mid
+ Low) | ‘Let’s
go’ (request with respect) |
sèí | (Low
+ High) | (‘Let’s
go’ (implying urgency and compulsion) |
ó: | (High
+ toned) | ‘Interjective
expression of wonderment’ |
| (High
+ Mid) | ‘expression
of wonderment as something happened as expected’ |
ô: | (High
+ Low) | ‘expression
of wonderment as something expected does not happen’ |
õ: | (High
+ Low + High) | ‘expression
of wonderment and anger as something |
õ: | (Low
+ High + Low) | ‘expression
of wonderment and insult as somebody cannot do something as expected’ |
mèì | (Low
- toned) | ‘Yes.
(It is)’ |
méí | (High) | ‘Is
it? (I don’t believe)’ |
méi | (High
+ Mid) | ‘Yes.
(It is. Don’t ask me again)’ |
mèí | (Low-High) | ‘Yes.
(It would. Let’s not worry)’ | 1.7.2.
Tonal change
in derivation The
tone of a root is not changeable in the process of inflection and compounding. However, when a root is subjected to derivation,
its tone is often changed obligatorily. The following examples illustrate tonal change in derivation of
noun from roots. Tonal change in derivation
is idiosyncratic and thus, no rule can be formulated.
Root |
|
Derived
noun |
|
thì | ‘to
die’ | à-thít | ‘death’ |
sà | ‘be
hot’ | à-sát | ‘heat’ |
pak | ‘be
wide’ | à-pàk | ‘width’ |
lèì-
šì | ‘to
love’ | lei-
šat | ‘love’ |
khón-
šì | ‘to
curse’ | khón-
šat | ‘curse’ |
làη-so | ‘be
proud’ | làη-sot | ‘pride’ |
m∂η-mü | ‘be
foolish’ | m∂η-mút | ‘foolishness’ |
A
more complicated tonal system might contain differential qualities of glides such
as differences of beginning-point height, differences in beginning and ending
points, differences of distance (slight fall; great fall), differences of time
(shorter time; longer time), differences in correlation between the time and the
distance of rise or fall (fast beginning rise with slow ending; slow beginning
fall with fast ending), and so on. 1.8
Syllabic structure Syllable may be defined as a unit of sound or sequence of sounds produced
with a single pulse of air pressure from the lungs. A syllable may form one word or part of a word. In Tangkhul-Naga, a simple vowel (vocalic unit),
a diphthong, a vocalic unit preceded by a consonant or consonant cluster, or a
vocalic unit preceded and followed by consonants, form a syllable.
Thus, Tangkhul-Naga syllable structure is : (C) (C) V (V) (C) Presence of a vowel is necessary in every syllabic structure. Permitted occurrence of initial and final consonants
and their clusters determine the syllabic boundary of the language. In close wyllables final consonants form the
syllabic boundary whereas in open syllables final vowel form the syllabic boundary.
The vowel constitutes the ‘peak’ of the syllable. A word level analysis of Tangkhul-Naga attests the following syllabic
patterns. There can be one t four phonemic
units in a syllable which can occur in their permissible order. (Syllabic sequence of VV type are diphthongs and therefore, unsegmentable.
[V represents semi-vowel]
(i) |
V |
i |
‘I’ |
*(ii) | VV | ∂̀i | ‘yes (I will)’ |
*(iii) | VVC | àíš | ‘expression of disbelieve or doubt’ |
(iv) | VV | yá | ‘right (hand/side)’ |
(v) | VVV | yàò | ‘sheep’ |
(vi) | VC | ot | ‘thing’ |
(vii) | CVV | mei | ‘fire’ |
(viii) | CVV | kwa | ‘pan (betel nut)’ |
*(ix) | CVVV | k***ao | ‘chicken’s crying sound in fright’ |
*(x) | CVVC | h∂̀ ís | ‘expression of disagreement, disbelieve, or rejection’ |
(xi) | CCV | ***k∂̀ | ‘No! (expression of unyielding)’ |
(xii) | CCVV | préi | ‘rt. of ‘married woman’ |
(xiii) | CCVC | kruη | ‘thundering sound’ | All
the above nine monosyllabic patterns, excluding the expressive words marked with
asterisk, can constitute syllabic unit of a disyllabic or polysyllabic word. 1.9
Morphophonemics Morphophonemic
adjustments or changes take place in the process of inflection, derivation, compounding
and reduplication. It reflects the relationship
of the phonemic structure of the language to its grammatical structure.
The morphophonemic patters of the language can be grouped as (a) phonologically
conditioned and (b) morphologically conditioned alterations. In
the following discussion, attempt is made to present all the rules of euphonic
combinations and morphological alterations or free variations under different
heads according to the nature of different morphophonemic changes. In many cases these may overlap and interact one another too.
[Symbols used under are : / for ‘or’; + for ‘followed by’; → for
‘is rewritten as’ ~ for ‘in free variation with; # for ‘word boundary’]. 1.9.1.
Phonologically conditioned alterations 1.9.1.1.
Suffixation of /∂̀/ The
most productive morphophonemic rules in the language are found in the suffixation
of /∂̀/, a homophonous morpheme for (1) non-future marker; (2) nominal
marker; (3) masculine marker; (4) attributive adjective marker.
The phonological changes depend upon the morphemic status of /∂̀/. Rules
in non-future forms of verbs :
(i) |
∂ |
→ |
i/V- # |
|
|
|
|
+ |
Back |
|
- |
Back |
|
|
|
|
- |
High |
|
+ |
Low |
| | | | - | Low | | | |
|
as in |
|
|
|
a) |
po + ∂̀ |
→ |
po – ì beg-NFUT
‘beg, begged’ |
|
(b) |
sá + à |
→ |
sá-ì do-NFUT
‘do, did’ |
(ii) |
∂ |
→ |
y∂/(V)V
- # |
|
|
|
|
+ |
Back |
|
|
|
|
+ |
High |
as in: |
|
|
|
|
(a) |
pí + ∂̀ |
→ |
pí-y∂̀ sleep-NFUT’sleep,
slept’ |
|
(b) |
sì + ∂̀ |
→ |
Bad-NFUT ‘is bad’ |
|
(c) |
théi+∂̀ |
→ |
théi+y∂̀ see-NFUT
‘see, saw’ |
(iii) |
∂ |
→ |
w∂/VV
- # |
|
|
|
|
+ |
Back |
|
|
|
|
+ |
High |
as in : |
|
|
|
|
(a) |
kào+∂̀ |
→ |
kào-w∂̀ thin-NFUT
‘is thin’ |
|
(b) |
teo-∂̀ |
→ |
teo-w∂̀ small-NFUT
‘is small’ |
| | | | | | | | | | |
if
/∂̀/is nominal, masculine, agentive or attributive adjective marker,
its phonemic status is always retained though it may affect the preceding phoneme.
[Prefix /k∂̀/or /kh∂̀/is the nominalizer
or non-finite marker].
(1) |
k∂̀-po NOMZ-beg |
‘to beg, begging’ |
| k∂̀-po-∂̀ | ‘one
who begs’ |
(2) | k∂̀-cik | ‘black,
blackness’ |
| k∂̀-cìk-∂̀ | ‘black,
blackness, something whivh is black, one (M) who is black’ |
1.9.1.2.
Affixation of /k∂/ /k∂̀/
is the nominalizer or non-finite marker. when
it is followed b a vowel or snorant it becomes /kh∂̀/
Rule
: |
|
|
|
k∂̀→kh∂̀/- | [Vowel]/[Sonorant]# | kh∂̀-m∂̀
η | ‘to
drink drinking’ |
k∂̀-pí | ‘to
sleep; sleeping’ | m∂η-kh∂̀-mü | ‘to
be foolish foolishness’ |
riη-k∂̀-phá | ‘to
be happy, happiness’ | kh∂̀-ló̀ | ‘to
buy buying’ |
k∂-sò | ‘to
praise, praising’ | kh∂̀-yam | ‘
to flee fleeing’ |
k∂̀-khop | ‘to
stitch, stitching’ | kh∂̀-òn | ‘to
change changing’ |
k∂̀-hòr | ‘to
shine, shining’ | | |
1.9.2.
Morphologically conditioned alterations 1.9.2.1.Contraction Vocalic
modification in a stem vowel is a very common phenomenon of morphological alterations.
contraction of the final two morphemes occurs optionally in deriving agentive
nouns or attributive adjectives. Examples
:
(1)
kh∂̀-lí |
NF-steal ‘to steal, stealing’ |
kh∂̀-lí-y∂̀ ~ kh∂̀-lé |
NF-steal-AGT NF-steal+AGT ‘thief’ |
(2)
m∂tùì- kh∂̀-mì |
speak-NF-give ‘to speak/speaking
for others’ |
m∂̀tùi- kh∂̀-mi-y∂̀ ~ m∂tù̀i-mè |
speak-NF-give-AGT speak-give + AGT ‘speakers’ |
(3) kh ∂ -ní ‘two’
k∂̀-ní-y∂̀ ~ k∂̀- kh ∂ té
NF-two-AFT/ATTRI NF-two +AGT/ATTRI
‘second’ |
(4)
m∂̀tì ‘four’
kh∂̀-m∂̀tì - y∂̀
~ kh∂̀-m∂̀tè
NF-four-AGT/ATTRI NF-four+AGT/ATTRI
‘fourth’ |
1.9.2.2.Changes
in nominalization Alteration
takes place in the process of nominalization of verbal stems ending in high vowel.
this is found only in the following few examples :
lèi-k∂̀-
šì |
‘love; loving’ |
→ |
lei-šat |
‘love’ |
rì η-k∂̀ -šì | ‘be/being
poor’ | → | rì
η-šat | ‘poverty’ |
là η-k∂̀-so | ‘be/being
proud’ | → | làη-sót | ‘pride’ |
m∂ η-kh∂̀-mü | ‘be/being
foolish’ | → | m∂η-mút | ‘foolishness’ |
Apart
from the above-mentioned alterations, there are a few other types of alterations
found in some constructions (derivation, inflection, compounding and reduplication).
They will be discussed further in the following chapters.
|