What
is Comprehension?
One of the earliest recorded studies of reading as reasoning was that of THORNDIKE
in 1917. The first factoral study of comprehension in reading was made by DAVIS
in 1941. A factoral study means that an attempt has been made to separate the
component parts of reading in order to discover what the statistical values are
of the various kinds of things one does that helps him to comprehend while reading.
For example, it is obvious that one of the factors in reading would be vocabulary.
What are the other factors? These are the kinds of questions that a factoral analysis
of reading attempts to answer. DAVIS's study indicated that reading behaviour
was related to the psychological aspects of reasoning as well as to the ocular
and mechanical. DAVIS hypothesized nine variables but found that only five met
his criteria, i.e., five were significant. They are given below:
1.
Vocabulary-knowledge of word meanings
2. Verbal reasoning-being able to reason
with words.
3. Sensitivity to implications-being able to understand implications,
or what a writer implies.
4. Following the structure of passage-to know how
it is structured and being able to follow the structure.
5. Recognizing the
literary techniques of the writer.
In
1946, THURSTON reanalyzed DAVIS's data and concluded that DAVIS had no statistical
claim for his findings and that only a single or general factor comprised reading
ability. HALL and ROBINSON in 1945 identified comprehension accuracy, rate of
inductive reading, word meaning, rate for reading unrelated facts and chart reading
skills. Another team identified only two factors-semantic difficulty, i.e., the
difficulty of the sentences, and word difficulty. In 1964, another team attempted
factor analysis and they found only two skills that were statistically defensible,
i.e., word knowledge and paragraph comprehension. In 1966, DAVIS did another factoral
study using his original hypothesis. He found again that five skills were significant.
The one that made up the greater part of the variance was again memory for word
meanings. He found that vocabulary made up 32 percent of what we mean by reading
comprehension. The second factor was drawing inferences 20 percent, i.e., to find
out what the author is saying 'between the lines'. Being able to follow the structure
of a passage was 14 percent. Being able to recognize the writer's purpose, attitude
and tone, was 11 percent. Ability to find answers explicit or in factual questions
10 percent. This makes up 87 percent of the total comprehension, the remaining
13 percent was made up of the other three factors he studied. Another team, as
late as 1969, reported a study of reading comprehension done in Iowa. This team
investigated eight factors as possible 'building blocks' in comprehension, but
found only four of these to be actually related to comprehension of reading.They
were (1) speed of reading; (2) ability to listen; (3) ability to classify the
words and (4) speed of noting details.A later study also found vocabulary to be
very significant, as was ability to note rhyming sounds.
There are taxonomic analyses also of reading. A taxonomy is a listing of all the
possible skills that are keen to exist in an orderly category of presentation.
One researcher hypothesized four basic steps to be present and further reasoned
that these four could be broken down into sub-factors. These four basic factors
were-
1)
literal comprehension with 15 sub-factors;
2) ability to re-organize with
13 sub-factors;
3) ability to evaluate with 5 sub-factors; and
4) ability
to appreciate literature with 4 sub-factors.
Another area of interest has been in an attempt to define comprehension as
that of building concepts. Words one hears or reads can help to give objects,
dimensions or attributes for classification and this classification is a part
of concept forming. One way of classifying is by defining a thing in terms of
its operation. For example, a bicycle-it gets people from one place to another.
If this is basic to comprehension, then a person comprehends by being able to
classify, to identify, to recall and to re-order and relate that which he has
read. Teaching for reading comprehension would then include training in identifying,
recording and classifying.
To
summarize, one way of defining comprehension is by seeing if there are certain
factors which are analyzable as being separate and discrete by the statistical
performance of factoral analysis. Reading can also be thought of as putting down
in logical sequential order all of the steps which could possibly go in to making
up comprehension or the way in which a person is able to get meaning from the
printed page. This is listing in a taxonomic or in an outline form all of the
steps in order and in sequence. A third way is to think of reading as a conceptual
act or as building concepts. If a concept helps to give each finite or individual
item within our environment a classification, then with them one is able to build
various kinds of organizational systems. Then if reading is a part of being able
to do this, as we learn to classify from our reading by learning to recall, identify,
order and relate, we have learned to comprehend.
These
are some of the ways people have looked at reading.
What
are the implications of these research studies in the writing of textbooks on
reading?
A
study of the various workbooks on reading indicates that some writers write workbooks
mostly after their own opinion and with little attempt to follow of what is known
of research. But some writers of workbooks have relied on research very heavily,
although they are in the minority.
Research
on Rate of Reading and Comprehension
The facts from the research on the rate of reading and comprehension indicates
that bright, fast readers do tend to comprehend better than bright slow readers.
That is, brighter, or very intelligent people comprehend better if they learn
to read faster. The duller student reverses this relationship. The faster he reads
the less he comprehends, for a person who is not very intelligent cannot learn
to read rapidly effectively. But the bright student can learn to read rapidly
effectively. On the other hand, in the contents of mathematics and science a low
negative relationship exists between the rate at which they are read and the amount
comprehend. That is, if one is going to read something highly complex and technical
such as science or mathematics, then one must read it slowly.
In other words, the more unfamiliar and complex a writing is, the more slowly
it must be read.
Questions
and Reading
Research has shown that the more questions that a person is able to ask relative
to what he is reading or learning the higher his comprehension will be. This,
as you know, is a part of our preview-skimming technique. That we teach previewing
and questioning before reading was included from basic fundamental research which
has shown that the more questions one is able to raise before reading the higher
his comprehension will be.
To summarize, while research is not inclusive and we do not have a concluding
statement on research in comprehension, we can say certainly that these studies
indicate that vocabulary is important, the ability to understand the various details
in a reading by answering questions is important, the ability to understand the
interferences that may be picked up in a reading are important, and that the ability
to read flexibly or with a fairly good rate in those kinds of readings which are
relevant to read rapidly are all necessary skills. Being able to ask questions
is also related to comprehension as is being able to understand the purpose of
the writer.
There are a variety of other factors that aid comprehension. Factors generally
considered under 'learning principles' are of great importance relative to what
one learns and remembers from a reading. First of all, motivation and drive are
fundamental, as is also the amount and nearness of reinforcement. Reinforcement
suggests that the reader gains some tangible good from his reading, and that this
positive effect follows fairly closely the reading act. In other words, the reader
finds reading useful, or it brings pleasure in one way or another. He is thus
reinforced to repeat the activity, in this case reading for meaning or comprehension.
Learning principles also show that one selects a task for learning that presents
some challenge, that one prefers a goal that is not instantly achieved but welcomes
a certain amount of frustration before the ultimate satisfaction of achievement.
Thus a reader, if he sees purpose in it, enjoys learning to read and comprehend
at increasingly more difficult levels. But again, he must feel rewarded for his
extra work, and reinforcement must be clear. The relevance of the reading must
be understood as helping him towards some clearly defined goal, helping him to
perform some tasks, or just because he really enjoys it. But reading, as a learned
act, must be seen as relevant and meaningful to the reader or he will not come
to learn it well.