Abbreviations:
KH = Kannada first language Hindi third language
HK = Hindi first
language Kannada third language
UH = Urdu first language Hindi third language
SH = Sanskrit first language Hindi third language
SK = Sanskrit first language
Kannada third language
TLH = Tamil first language Hindi third language
O.B. = Other Bilinguals
O.M. = Other Multilinguals
For purposes of comparison
of the language achievement of monolinguals and bilinguals, only the KH and UH
groups were considered. Since most of the HK groups students were bilinguals,
this group was not considered for purposes of comparing the achievement of monolinguals
and bilinguals. Of the 427 subjects covered in the survey about 43 per cent spoke
more than one language and thus were bilinguals. A little over 56 per cent were
monolinguals. Out of the monolinguals, about 83.5 per cent were Kannada mother
tongue speakers, and 14.8 percent Urdu mother tongue speakers.
83
per cent of the monolinguals studied Kannada as first language and Hindi
as third language and 14.88 per cent studied Urdu as first language and Hindi
as third language. This entire 14.88 per cent consists of Urdu Mother tongue speakers.
There were however, some Urdu Mother tongue speakers who belong to the KH group.
Among the bilinguals, 64.32 per cent studied Kannada as first language and Hindi
as third language; 11.89 per cent studied Urdu as first language and Hindi as
third language and 22.16 per cent studied Hindi as first language and Kannada
as third language. Here also, all the students who studied Urdu as first language
were bilinguals with Urdu as mother tongue.
(a)
Effect of bilingualism on overall performance and language achievement
It
may be observed form Table 2 that in almost all the cases the average marks in
overall performance and in language achievement of bilinguals are higher than
those of monolinguals. But when Smirnov's test (which is used to test whether
the two populations are identical or not) is used (Table 4) for the equality of
two distributions form which the two sample were drawn, it is revealed that the
monolinguals and bilinguals do not differ in their achievement. The exception
to this is the difference in the case of the third language of single group viz.,
KH group, where bilinguals show better performance than monolinguals.
Out
of the 242 monolinguals, 235 (97.10 per cent) have opted for their mother tongue
as first language. Among the 185 bilinguals, on the other hand, only 41 (22.16
per cent) have opted for their mother tongue as first language. This means that
the achievement of bilinguals in a language which is not their mother tongue has
a slight edge over the achievement of monolinguals in their mother tongue. This
could be interpreted as the better language achievement for the bilinguals.
The distribution of the marks among the subjects of the different groups
of monolinguals and bilinguals can be seen from the Table 3(a) to 3(c) and form
graphs 1 to 12. The slight but statistically insignificant edge of the bilinguals
over the monolinguals in their achievement can be seen more clearly in the graphs.
There is no significant difference between the achievement of subjects in
the 8th and the 9th classes as can be observed form Table 2. However, the variation
in the achievement of bilinguals in the two years is relatively more than that
of the monolinguals and the achievement is higher in the 9th class.
Graph
1
Graph 12
TABLE 2
Means and Standard Deviations of the Marks of the Students of Different
Language Option
Groups
in Eighth and Ninth Classes
Group
|
Subject
|
Mono/Bi/
In all |
Eighth
Class |
Ninth
Class |
Mean
|
S.D.
|
Mean
|
S.D.
|
|
Overall
Performance |
Mono
|
44.3
|
9.509
|
46.2
|
11.737
|
Bi
|
45.9
|
11.306
|
49.1
|
12.249
|
In all
|
44.8
|
10.402
|
47.3
|
12.024
|
KH
|
First
Language |
Mono
|
46.4
|
11.420
|
47.8
|
11.407
|
Bi
|
47.0
|
12.434
|
48.7
|
11.194
|
In all
|
46.6
|
11.799
|
48.2
|
11.331
|
Third
Language |
Mono
|
38.7
|
15.070
|
40.8
|
18.234
|
Bi
|
43.7
|
15.905
|
47.3
|
17.819
|
In all
|
40.5
|
15.561
|
43.2
|
18.341
|
Overall
Performance |
Mono
|
41.2
|
4.195
|
40.6
|
3.760
|
Bi
|
44.9
|
9.014
|
48.3
|
5.523
|
In all
|
42.5
|
6.812
|
43.5
|
6.081
|
UH
|
First
Language |
Mono
|
51.1
|
11.137
|
54.6
|
10.285
|
Bi
|
55.0
|
15.862
|
58.4
|
15.784
|
In all
|
52.5
|
13.187
|
56.1
|
12.632
|
Third
Language |
Mono
|
47.9
|
13.878
|
41.8
|
12.454
|
Bi
|
49.6
|
17.262
|
44.7
|
13.658
|
In all
|
48.5
|
15.243
|
42.9
|
12.901
|
Overall
Performance |
Bi
|
54.0
|
11.732
|
61.1
|
12.226
|
HK
|
First
Language |
Bi
|
49.5
|
14.442
|
55.3
|
13.892
|
Third
Language |
Bi
|
44.7
|
11.894
|
60.9
|
16.618
|
Table
3 (a) and Table 3 (b)
(b)
Comparison of overall performance, first and third language achievement
Comparison
between overall performance and achievement in first and third languages was made
for the three L.O. groups (KH, UH, HK). First language performance is better than
the overall performance and third language performance in the KH and UH groups
but not in the HK group.
In
the UH group the performance in the first language is very high. About two-thirds
of the students in the 9th class and half of the students in the 8th class of
the UH group have got more than 50 per cent of marks in their first language (i.e.,
Urdu). In the KH group slightly more than one-third of students in the 9th class
and exactly one-third students in the 8th class got more than 50 per cent marks
in their first language, (i.e., Kannada). Though the corresponding figures of
first language (i.e., Hindi) performance for the HK group are more or less the
same as in the UH group, its overall performance, particularly in the 9th class,
is far better than its first language performance. 84 per cent of the students
of the HK group in the 9th class have got more than 50 per cent of marks in overall
performance and more than 50 per cent of the students of this group in the 8th
class have got more than 50 per cent of marks in overall performance. In the KH
and UH groups, on the other hand, at the most one-third and one-eighth of the
students respectively, have got more than 50 per cent marks in overall performance.
These differences have been represented in Tables 3(a) to 3(c) and in graphs 13-21.
The reasons for these differences must have been that the performance of the HK
group in non-language subjects like Mathematics, Science and Social Studies is
better than the performance of the other groups in these subjects, since there
is no significant difference in the language achievement between the three groups.
It follows form this that the performance of HK group in non-language subjects
is better than its performance in languages.
The
marks of overall performance include the marks in first and third languages. Therefore,
when overall performance is compared with language achievement, the comparison
is not between independent entities. In order to find out the influence of language
achievement in overall performance, the various regression equations of the overall
performance, achievement in first and third languages with respect to different
groups and classes were found out (Table 5). The regression equations partial
out the other subjects like Mathematics, Science, Social Studies and English.
Equations in Table 5 show that for every one per cent of increase in the first
language there will be 0.4453 per cent rise in the overall percentage and every
one per cent rise in the third language gives 0.2349 per cent rise in the overall
percentage, provided there is no change in the marks in other subjects. The influence
of language achievement on the overall performance in the KH and the HK groups
in both classes is more or less the same but in the UH group it is different.
For one per cent rise in the marks of the first language of the UH group, there
is 0.3 per cent rise in the overall performance, and for one per cent rise in
the marks of the third language there is 0.17 per cent and 0.31 percent rises
in the overall performance in the eighth and ninth classes respectively. The UH
group thus differs significantly from the other groups with regard to the influence
of language achievement in overall performance.
From
equations IA and IB, it may be observed that for KH group the increases in the
overall performance of eighth class are 0.6408, 0.2340 for monolinguals and 0.5664,
0.1976 for bilinguals for one per cent rise in the first and the third languages
respectively. Similarly, the corresponding figures for the ninth class are 0.5663,
0.2834, 0.5568 and 0.2869 respectively. That is, the influence of the achievement
in languages on the overall performance is more in the case of monolinguals. Also,
the influence of language achievement is higher in the ninth class.
TABLE
4
Smirnov's Test for the Equality of the Distributions of Mono and Bilinguals
of UH Group
Subject |
Class |
Variables |
Static |
Calculated
Value | Tabulated
Value | Decision
Regarding Distribution | Remarks |
Two-Tail |
One-Tail |
Overall
Performance | Eighth |
Mono and
Bi | T |
0.2569 |
0.4007 |
0.3594 |
Mono=Bi |
No difference
in Performance |
T1 |
|
Ninth |
Mono and
Bi | T |
0.3434 |
0.3680 |
0.3302 |
Mono=Bi |
,, |
T1 |
First
Language | Eighth |
Mono and
Bi | T |
0.2952 |
0.4007 |
0.3594 |
Mono=Bi |
,, |
T1 |
|
Ninth |
Mono and
Bi | T |
0.1818 |
0.3680 |
0.3302 |
Mono=Bi |
,, |
T1 |
Third
Language | Eighth |
Mono and
Bi | T |
0.1771 |
0.4007 |
0.3594 |
Mono=Bi |
,, |
T1 |
|
Ninth |
Mono and
Bi | T |
0.1767 |
0.3602 |
0.3302 |
Mono=Bi |
,, |
T1 |
Note:
T = Max [Sn1-Sn2]
T1 = Max (Sn1-Sn2)
TABLE
5
Regression
Equations of Overall Performance and Achievement in First and Third Languages
Variable
|
8th
Class |
9th
Class |
Mono
(KH) |
X1
= 0.6408X2 + 0.2340X3 + 5.5111 |
X1
= 0.5663X2 + 0.2834X3 + 7.5681 |
Bi
(KH) |
X1
= 0.5664X2 + 0.1976X3 + 10.6441-IA
|
X1
= 0.5568X2 + 0.2869X3 + 8.4135 |
KH
|
X1
= 0.4453X2 + 0.2349X3 + 14.5356 |
X1
= 0.5761X2 + 0.3091X3 + 6.1789 |
UH
|
X1
= 0.3114X2 + 0.1721X3 + 17.8046-*
|
X1
= 0.2983X2 + 0.3101X3 + 13.4621 |
HK
|
X1
= 0.6447X2 + 0.1407X3 + 15.7981 |
X1
= 0.5794X2 + 0.2784X3 + 12.1046 |
Note:
X1 denotes overall performance
X2
denotes first language performance
X3
denotes third language performance
*
This is equation I
TABLE
6
Correlations Between the Languages and Overall Performances of the Students
of Different Groups
Group
|
Mono
/ Bi |
Subjects
|
Eighth
Class |
Ninth
Class |
1
|
2
|
3
|
4
|
5
|
|
Mono
|
|
0.7800
|
0.7979
|
|
0.6345
|
0.7497
|
|
0.4229
|
0.5623
|
|
0.5295
|
0.5543
|
|
0.5267
|
0.5695
|
|
0.4087
|
0.7040
|
KH
|
Bi
|
|
0.6946
|
0.7655
|
|
0.4382
|
0.7300
|
|
0.2571
|
0.6115
|
|
0.5462
|
0.6056
|
|
0.5734
|
0.6324
|
|
0.4778
|
0.6785
|
Total
|
|
0.7157
|
0.7819
|
|
0.5597
|
0.7467
|
|
0.3519
|
0.5069
|
|
0.5356
|
0.5777
|
|
0.5442
|
0.5976
|
|
0.4371
|
0.6926
|
HK
|
|
|
0.8483
|
0.8573
|
|
0.4469
|
0.7246
|
|
0.3834
|
0.5259
|
|
0.7869
|
0.7281
|
|
0.6579
|
0.6710
|
|
0.7385
|
0.7142
|
UH
|
|
|
0.6476
|
0.5486
|
|
0.5166
|
0.6135
|
|
0.3176
|
0.2358
|
|
-0.1125
|
0.0866
|
|
0.2644
|
0.2219
|
|
-0.0555
|
0.2279
|
Note:
OP is overall performance
Because the groups were very small, separate
correlations for mono and bilinguals of UH and HK groups were not given.
It may be observed form Table 6 that the correlation between first language
and overall performance is more than 0.54 in all the cases and the maximum (0.857)
is in the HK group and minimum (0.548) in the UH group for the ninth class. For
the eighth class, the correlation between first language achievement and overall
performance is more than 0.64 in all the cases and maximum (0.848) is in the HK
group and minimum (0.648) in the UH group. Similarly, the correlations between
the overall performance and the third language performance for KH, HK and UH groups
are 0.5597. 0.4469, 0.5166 in the eighth class and 0.7467, 0.7246 and 0.6135 in
the ninth class, respectively.
It
may also be seen from Table 6 that the correlations between the first language
achievement and the third language achievement are very low compared to the other
correlations. In the case of the UH group one would expect a high correlation
between Urdu, the first language, and Hindi, the third language. But it is not
so here.
The
correlations between the first and the third language achievement and the overall
performance were also tested by the 'Wilcoxon signed rank test' and the decisions
are given in Tables 7(a) to 7(c). As found earlier, it can be seen here also [Table
7(a) to 7(c)] that the average performance in first language is better than the
overall performance in all the cases except for HK group. The third language performance
in the ninth class is better than the performance in the eighth class. The 'decision'
column in Tables 7(a) to 7(c) also gives whether the average performance is lower
or higher
(c)
Language achievement in relation to the achievement in non-language subjects
The correlations between achievement in first language and other non-language
subjects like Mathematics (M), Science (S) and Social Studies (SS) were calculated
and are given in Table 6. It may be seen that the correlations in the case of
KH and HK groups are very significant. The correlations of HK group are the highest.
The actual values lie between 0.5356 and 0.7869 for Mathematics, between 0.5442
and 0.6710 for Science and between 0.4371 and 0.7385 for Social Studies. In the
eighth class, the correlations between the first language and Mathematics and
Social Studies of the UH group are negative but all the correlations of this group
are very insignificant. In the 9th class the correlations for the UH group are
also insignificant but all are positive.
(d)
Comparison of the achievements of KH, UH and HK groups
The comparison
of the achievements of different groups was made by drawing the 'frequency polygons'
of their distributions as shown in graphs 22-27. It was also tested for the differences
between the groups by using Smirnov's two sample test. It may be observed from
both the graphs and the Smirnov's test that the performance of HK group is better
than the other two groups. The performance of the UH group in Hindi (the third
language) is better than that of the KH group. The overall performance of the
UH group is low eventhough its first language performance is far better than the
other groups.
(e)
Comparison of the achievements of different categories of bilinguals
The
different categories of bilinguals are Kannada and Telugu, Kannada and Urdu, Kannada
and Malayalam, Kannada Tulu, Kannada Marathi, and Kannada Tamil bilinguals. The
equality of the average performance of the different categories of bilinguals
in the first language, the third language, etc., was tested by using Smirnov's
two sample test. It is observed that all the bilinguals groups do not differ significantly
in overall performance as well as in first and third language achievement.
(f)
Individual variation among the subjects
From Table 2 it is observed that
the standard deviations among the marks of bilinguals are greater than those of
monolinguals in all cases. For all the groups the variation in the first language
achievement in the eighth class is greater than that of ninth class variation.
For KH and HK groups, the variation in the third language achievement and overall
performance in the eighth class are less than that of ninth class. But for UH
group, the variation in the third language of the eighth class is more than the
variation in the ninth class of the same group. For all the groups (KH, HK, UH),
the variation in the overall performance is less than the variations in the first
and third language achievements. The maximum variation is found in case of third
language (Hindi) achievement.
(g)
Comparison of the achievements of the students with different media of instruction
Earlier studies (Anand 1971) have shown that educational achievement is better
when the medium of instruction is the mothertongue. In this study, as a secondary
interest, the performances of the Kannada medium students of KH group and the
English medium students of the same group were compared and tested for their disparity.
The comparison was also made by drawing the 'Percentage polygons of their distributions
as shown in graphs 34-39. The performance differences of the student sin the two
media were tested by using Smirnov's two sample test for the identity of the two
distributions from which the two samples were drawn. Tables (9a) and 9(b) show
that the students with English medium do better than the Kannada medium students
in both the first and the third languages in the eighth and the ninth classes.
Their overall performance is also better than the Kannada medium students. This
is also true in the case of mother tongue groups. This observation must be verified
by a larger and more controlled survey.
(h)
The effect of socio-economic background on language achievement
Anand
(1971) has shown that the socio-economic factors influence the academic attainment
of students to a great extent. This study also substantiates this finding as far
as the overall performance is concerned, but not in the case of language achievement.
The parental occupation and education do not seem to affect the student's language
achievement except in the case of students whose parents are teachers.
Conclusion
1. There is no significant difference between the first language achievement
of monolinguals and bilinguals though the bilinguals have a slight edge-over over
the monolinguals in their achievement in the first language.
2.
In the case of third language (Hindi) performance, the bilinguals differ form
the monolinguals. Bilinguals have done better (44.61 per cent) than the monolinguals
(40.10 per cent). The languages of the major bilingual groups are Telugu and Kannada,
Tamil and Kannada, Urdu and Kannada, Marathi and Kannada and Tulu and Kannada.
Among these bilingual groups, the third language (Hindi) performance of each group
is equal. Among the monolinguals, Urdu mother tongue students' performance in
the third language (Hindi) is better (47.9 per cent) than the Kannada mother tongue
students' performance (38.7 per cent).
3.
The overall performance of monolinguals and bilinguals does not differ significantly.
4.
Language achievement has high positive correlation with the overall performance
in the case of monolinguals, except in the case of the UH group of the monolinguals.
Its performance in the first language (Urdu) is much higher than their overall
performance. Given conclusion 3, this implies that in the case of bilinguals,
achievement in non-language subjects has high positive correlation with the overall
performance.
TABLE
8 Smirnov's Test for the Equality of the Distributions of the Students from Different
Groups
Class
|
Subject
|
Variable
|
Statistic
|
Calculated
Value |
Tabulated
Value |
Decision
Regarding Distributions |
Conclusion
|
Two-
tail |
One-tail
|
|
Overall
Performance |
KH
and UH |
T
T”
|
0.1347
|
0.2081
|
0.1866
|
KH=UH
|
No
significant difference between the KH and UH groups regarding their OP
|
|
|
KH
and HK |
T
T1
|
0.4166
|
0.2267
|
0.2034
|
KH>HK
|
The
OP of HK group is better than that of KH group |
|
|
UH
and HK |
T
T1
|
0.5005
|
0.2865
|
0.2570
|
UH>HK
|
The
Op of HK group is better than that of UH group |
Eighth
|
First
Language |
KH
and UH |
T
T”
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
KH
and HK |
T’
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
UH
and HK |
T’
|
|
|
|
|
|
5.
The average first language performance of both monolinguals and bilinguals is
better than their average overall performance except in the case of HK group.
(Only the bilinguals of this group are included for analysis since the monolinguals
of this group are not significant in number).
6.
The different bilingual groups do not differ significantly in their performance
in all subjects.
7.
The different language option groups (KH, UH, HK) do not differ significantly
in all subjects except the HK group whose performance in non-language subjects
like Mathematics, Science and Social Studies is far better than its performance
in languages and consequently is better than the performance of other groups in
these subjects.
8.
Individual variations in the achievement of bilinguals are more than those of
monolinguals.
9.
Language achievement of all the students in the ninth class is better than their
achievement in the eighth class.
10.
The English medium students have done better in all subjects than the Kannada
medium students with similar socio-economic background.
11.
Language achievement is not significantly influenced by parental occupation and
education except for the fact that the language achievement of teachers' children
is slightly better than that of the others.
APPENDIX
I
CONFIDENTIAL LANGUAGE ACHIEVEMENT STUDY
STUDENT PROFORMA1.
Name of the Student :
2.
Sex : Boy / Girl
3.
Age :
4.
Class Studying :
5.
Medium of instruction :
6.
Mother tongue :
(a) Mother's :
(b) Father's :
(c) Language spoken
at home :
7.
(a) Whether the family has migrated? Yes / No
8.
Religion and Caste
9.
Language (speak, read and write)
Language
|
Speak
|
Read
|
Write
|
Examinations
Passed |
Kannada
Marathi Telugu Urdu Hindi Others
(Specify)
|
|
|
|
|
10. Educational levels of parents: (a)
Father: Nil/ Below Middle / Middle/ Matric/ Inter or Higher Secondary or P.U.C.
/ Graduation / Post Graduation.
(b)
Mother: Nil/ Below Middle / Middle/ Matric / Inter or Higher Secondary or P.U.C./
Graduation / Post Graduation.
11.
Occupation of Parents:
(a) Father:
(b) Mother:
12.
Classes Studied:
Year
|
Class
|
First
language |
Second
language |
Third
language |
Fourth
language |
1971 1972 1973
|
|
|
|
|
|
13. Marks obtained in Annual examinations:
Subject
|
1971
|
1972
|
1973
|
Maximum
Marks |
Marks
Obtained |
Maximum
Marks |
Marks
Obtained |
Maximum
Marks |
Marks
Obtained |
English Kannada Marathi Telugu Urdu Tamil Hindi Sanskrit Maths Social
Studies Science Other
(Specify) In
All |
APPENDIX
II
[SELECTED BIBLIOGRAPHY]
1.
Anand, C.L., A study of the effect of Socio-Economic Environment and Medium of
Instruction on the Mental abilities and the Academic achievement of children in
Mysore State, Ph.D. thesis in Education (unpublished: 1972), Mysore University,
Mysore.
2. Arsenian Seth, Bilingualism and Mental Development, New York,
1937.
3.
Bilingualism in the Post-War World, Psychological Bulletin,
42, 65-86 (1945).
4. Aucamp, (Anna) J., Bilingual Education and Nationalism
with special reference to South Africa, Pretoria, 1926.
5. Babin, Partick,
Bilingualism - A Bibliography. Cambridge, Massachusetts, Harvard Graduate School
of Education, May 8, 1968, 30 pp. Unpublished.
6. Barke and Williams, A further
study of the comparative intelligence of children in certain bilingual and monolingual
schools in South Wales. Brit. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1938.
7.
Barker, George, C., Growing up in a Bilingual community, The Kiva 17, 17-32, 1951.
8. Bossard, James, H.S., The Bilingual Individual as a Person - Linguistic
Identification with Status, American Sociological Review, 10, 699-709, 1945.
9. A Bulletin of Bilingual Studies, Published by the Bilingualism Committee to
the University of Wales, No.1, October, 1936, (mimeographed); No.2, June, 1938;
No.3, June, 1939.
10. Buros, Oskar Krisen, (ed.); Third Mental Measurements
Yearbook, New Brunswick (N.J.), 1949.
11. Carrow, Sister Mary, A., Linguistic
Functioning of Bilingual and Monolingual children, Journal of Speech and Hearing
Disorders, Vol. XXII, pp. 371-380, 1957.
12. Chen, Elinor Yuk Lin, A study
of sentences by the Bilingual Child of Chinese Ancestry Unpublished M.A. Thesis,
Honolulu, 1935.
12.A. Chickermane, Dr V., Impact of Bilingualism on the progress
of Children in primary schools in Rural areas, NCERT, 1971.
13. Christopherson,
Paul, Bilingualism, London, 1948.
14. Coale, Willis Branson, Successful Practices
in the Teaching of English to Bilingual Children in Hawaii (-U.S. Department of
the Interior, Office of Education, Bulletin, 1937, 14), Washington, 1938.
15. Coleman, W. and Cureton, E.E., Intelligence and achievement; The Jangle fallacy
again Educ. Psychol. Measmt., 1954, 14, pp. 347-351.
15A. Conover, W.J.,
Practical Nonparametric Statistics, John Williamsons, New York, 1971.
16.
Cooper, James, G., Predicting school Achievement for Bilingual pupils, The Journal
of Educational Psychology, Vol. XLIX, No. 1, (Feb. 1958), pp. 31-36, 8 references.
17. Darcy, N.T., The effect of Bilingualism upon the Measurement of the Intelligence
of Children of Preschool Age, Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Fordham University,
1945.
18.
Review of the literature on the Effects of Bilingualism
upon the Measurement of Intelligence Journal of Genetic Psychology, 82, 21-57,
1953.
19. Davies, M. and Hughes, A.G., An Investigation into the comparative
intelligence and attainments of Jewish and Non-Jewish Children, British Journal
of Psychology (General section), 18, 134-46, 1927.
20. Diebold, A. Richard,
Incipient Bilingualism, Language, XXXVII, (1961a), pp. 97-112.
21.
Bilingualism and Biculturalism in Huave Community. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
Department of Anthropology, Yale University, 1961b.
22.
Mexican and
Guatemalan Bilingualism, Frank A. Rice, ed. Study of the Role of Second Languages
in Asia, Africa and Latin America (Washington, Centre for Applied Lingualistics,
1962), pp. 26-33.
23. Dodd, Stuart, C., On Measuring Languages, Journal of
the American Statistical Association, 44, 77-88, (1949).
24. Ervin, S.M.
and Osgood, C.E., Second Language Learning and Bilingualism, Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology (Supplement), (1954), 49, 139-146.
25. Eswara, H.S.,
Language behaviour in relation to social acceptability.
26. Fishman, Joshuaa,
Degree of Bilingualism in a Yiddish School and Leisure Time Activities, Journal
of Social Psychology, 36, 155-65, 1952.
27.
Bilingualism in the Barrio,
Indiana University Publications, 7, 1971.
28. Friedrich, Paul, Languages
and Politics in India, Daedalus, XCI, pp. 543-559, (1962).
29. Great Britain,
Ministry of Education, Welsh Department, Bilingualism in the Secondary School
in Wales, Y Broblem Ddwyiethog yn yr Ysgol Uwchradd Yng Nghymru (= Pamphlet 4),
London, 1949.
30. Hall, Roberta, (Jr), Bilingualism and Applied Linguistics,
Zeitschrifit wFilr Phoneitk und allgemeine sprachwissenschaft, 6, 13-30, 1952.
31. Hasselmo, Nils, American Swedish: A study in Bilingualism, Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, Department of Linguistics, Harvard University, 1961.
32. Haugen, Einar, Problems of Bilingualism, Lingua 2, 271-90, 1950.
33.
Bilingualism in the Americas: A bibliography and research Guide (Alabama, University
of Alabama Press), 1956.
34.
The Norwegian Language in America: A
study in Bilingual Behaviour, 2 Volumes (Philadelphia, University of Pennsylvania
Press, 1953).
35. Herrick, Virgile and Leland, B. Jacobs, Children and the
Language Arts, Prentice Hall, (1955).
36. Hill, H.S., The effects of bilingualism
on the measured intelligence of elementary school children of Italian Parentage,
Journal of Experimental Education, 5, pp. 75-79, 1936.
37. Johnson, Granville,
B., (Jr), The relationship existing between Bilingualism and Racial Attitudes,
Journal of Educational Psychology, 42, 357-65, (1951).
38.
Bilingualism
as measured by a Reaction-time technique and the relationship between a language
Intelligent Quotient, Journal of Genetic Psychology, 82, 3-9, 1953.
39. Jones,
W.R., A critical study of bilingualism and non-verbal intelligence, Brit. J. Educ.
Psychology, 30, pp. 71-76, (1960).
40.
and Stewart, W.A.C., Bilingualism
and Verbal Intelligence, Beitish Journal of Psychology, (Stastical Section), 4,
3-8, (1951).
41. Kelly, L.G., Description and measurement of Bilingualism.
An International Seminar, (1970).
42. Khubchandani, L.M., Bilingual interference
in Language Learning.
43. Lagarde-Quost, P.H.J., The bilingual citizen, Britain
today, 140 (December 1947), 15-9, 141, 13-7, January 1948.
44. Lambert, W.E.,
A Social Psychology of bilingualism, Journal of Social issues, 23, 91-109, (1967).
45.
Linguistic manifestations of bilingualism, American Journal of
Psychology, 72, 77-82, (1959).
46.
Measurement of the Linguistic Dominance
of Bilinguals, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, L pp. 197-200, (1955).
47. Leopold, Werner, F., Speech Development of a Bilingual Child, 4 Vols, Evanston
(III), 1939-50.
48. Levinson, B.M., A comparison of the performance of bilingual
and monolingual native-born Jewish preschool children of traditional parentage
on four intelligence tests, Journal of Clinical Psychology, 15, pp. 74-76, (1959).
49. Lewis, Bilingualism and Bilingual Education in the Soviet Union.
50. Lowie, Robert H., A case of Bilingualism, Word, 1, 249-59, (1945).
51.
Lynn, Konda, Bilingualism in the South West, Quarterly Journal of Speech, 31,
175-89, (1945).
52. Mackey, W.F., Bilingualism as a World Problem, Montreal,
Harvest House.
53.
Bilingual interference; its analysis ad measurement,
Journal of Communication, 15, 239-249, (1965).
54.
The description
of bilingualism, Canadian Journal of linguistics, 7, 51-85, (1962).
55.
The measurement of bilingual behaviour, Canadian Psychologist, 7, 75-92, (1966).
56. Mackey, W.F. and Moonan, G.A., An experiment in bilingual education,
English Language Teaching, 6/2 (Summer 1952), No.4, pp.125-132.
57. Macnamara,
John, The bilinguals Linguistic Performance. A Psychological overview. The Journal
of Social issues, 1967.
58.
The linguistic independence of bilinguals,
Journal of verbal learning and verbal Behaviour, 6, 729-736, 1967b.
59. Malherbe,
Ernest Gideon, The bilingual school; a study of Bilingualism in South Africa,
Johannesburg, 1934; Afrikaans Edition, Capetown, 1943.
60. Mathai, S., The
future of three Language formula, Secondary Education, October, pp. 9-10, 1959.
61. Mikes, M., Linguistic Inference in a bilingual community.
62. O'Doherty,
E.F., Bilingualism; Educational Aspects, Advancing Science, 56, pp. 287-90, (1958).
63. Padma, Y.V., Assessment of Intelligence among mono and multilinguals
(Dissertation, 1972), Department of Psychology, University of Mysore, Mysore.
64. Peal, Elizabeth and lambert, W.E., The relation of bilingualism to Intelligence,
Psychological monograph, 76, No. 27, (1962).
65. Pieris, Ralph, Bilingualism
and Cultural Marginality, British Journal of Sociology, 2, 328-39, (1951).
66. Pintner, R., (Udolf), and Arsenian, Seth, The relation of Bilingualism to
verbal Intelligence and School adjustment, Journal of Educational Psychology.
51, 255-63, (1937).
67. Plant, Problems of Linguistic Interference in the
Education of Bilingual Childrens.
68. Puitner, R. and Arsenian, S., The relation
of bilingualism to verbal intelligence and school adjustment, Journal of Educational
Research, 31, pp. 255-263, (1937).
69. Rao, T.S., Academic Achievement of
Bilingual children, Indian Psychological Review, Vol. VIII, No. 1, pp. 8-9, (July
1971).
70. Raubicheck, Letitia, The Psychology of Multilingualism, Volta
Review 36, 17-20, 57f, (1934).
71. Rayfield, R.J., The Language of a Bilingual
Community, JL Series Practica 77, pp. 118, 1970.
72. Read, A(Llan) W(alker),
Bilingualism in the Middle colonies, 1725-1775, American Speech 12, 93-100, (1937).
73. Roberts, Murat, H., The problem of the Hybrid Language, Journal of English
and Germanic Philology 38, 23-41, (1937).
74. Rubin, John, Bilingualism in
paraguay, Anthropological Linguistics, IV, pp. 52-58, (1962).
75.
National Bilingualism in Paraguay, Mouton, 1968.
76. Saer, D.L., The effect
of Bilingualism on Intelligence, British Journal of Psychology 14, 25-38.
77. Sear, Hywella, A., Experimental Enquiry into the Education of Bilingual Peoples,
Education in a Changing Commonwealth, London, 116-21, (1931).
78. Singer,
H., bilingualism and elementary education, Moderna Language Journal 40, 444-58,
(1956).
79. Sissons, Charles, B., Bilingual schools in Canada, London, 1917.
80. Smith, Christina, Mental Testing of Hebridean Children in Gaelic and
English, London, 1948.
81. Smith, Madorah, E., A study of five bilingual
children from the same family, Child Development 6, 184-7, (1931).
82.
A study of the speech of Eight Bilingual Children of the same family, Child Development
6, 19-25, (1935).
83.
Some Light on the Problem of Bilingualism as
found from a study of the Progress in mastery of English Among Pre-school Children
of Non-American Ancestry in hawaii, Genetic Psychology Monographs 21, 119-284,
(1939).
84.
Measurement of Vocabularies of young bilingual children
in both languages use, Journal of Genetical Psychology, 74, pp. 305-310, (1949).
85.
World variety as a measure of bilingualism in pre-school children,
Journal of Genetical Psychology, 90, pp. 143-150, (1957).
86. Spoerl, Dorothy
Tilden, Bilinguality and Emotional Adjustment, Journal of Abnormal and Social
Psychology 38, 37-57, (1946).
87.
The Academic and Verbal Adjustment
of College Age Bilingual students, Journal of Genetic Psychology 64, 139-57, (1944).
88. St Denis, Henri, Bilingual Education, Canadian School Journal 12, 213-7,
246, (1934).
89. Theodore Andersson and Mildred Boyer, Bilingual Schooling
in the United States, Southwest Educational Development Laboratory Austin, Texas,
January, 1970.
90. Travis, L.E., Johnson, W. and Shover, J., The Relation
of Bilingualism to stuttering, Journal of Speech Disorders 2, 185-9, (1937).
91. Weinreich, U., Functional aspects of Indian bilingualism, Word 13, 203-233,
(1957).
92.
Languages in a contact, Mouton, 1970.
93.
Present day approaches to the study of bilingualism. Unpublished M.A. Thesis,
Columbia University, 1949.
94.
Research Problems in Bilingualism with
special reference to Switzerland Unpublished Dissertation, Columbia University,
1951, available on Microfilm; summary in Dissertation, abstracts 12-418f, (1952).
95. West, Michael, Bilingualism (with Special reference to Bengal), Calcutta,
1926.
96. Whitney, W (Lliam) D (Wight), On Mixture in Language, Transactions
of the American Philological Association 12, 1-26, (1881).
97. Yammamoto,
Kaoru, Bilingualism-A brief review, Mental Hygiene, Vol. XLVIII, No. 3 pp. 468-47,
10 reference, July (1964).