Prev
| Home | Next
In
one of his recent articles Chomsky (1981b) talks about two major shifts of focus:
the first was a shift of focus from language to grammar which started in 1955
with the publication of "The logical structure of linguistic theory"
and spanned over a period of twenty-five years, before it found a theoretical
formulation in Chomsky's book 'Rules and Representations'. The second shift of
focus was from rule-systems, as explicated in 'Syntactic Structures' (1975) and
'Aspects of a Theory of Syntax' (1965), to system of principles such as the Binding
Theory and the Projection Principle ('Lectures on Government and Binding', 1981a).
The first of the two shifts of focus mentioned above, i.e., from language to grammar,
considers language as derivative and secondary in status. According to Chomsky
the notion of language, as compared to the notion of grammar, is a more abstract
one, and is even epiphenomenal in existence. For instance, when we talk about
Hindi language, we are led to regional and sociolectal variants, standard and
non-standard forms, different styles and registers, etc. Since all these are covered
under the rubric of language (Hindi), one has to generalize at different levels,
on the basis of these variable data, in order to arrive at the notion of a language.
When talking about Hindi, for instance one can have three levels of generalizations
(a) Hindi, which ahs in its ambit the different dialects, e.g., Braj (with literary
giants like Surdas and Bihari), Avadhi (with literary geniuses like Jayasi and
Tulsi Das), and Khari Boli (with literary artists like Jai Shankar Prasad and
Nirala). These dialects collectively constitute the institutional reality of Hindi
language; (b) a narrower view of Hindi, which embraces the regionally unmarked
koine of India, and functionally operates as a link language, a sort of pan-Indian
language; and (c) the view of Hindi as a cultivated form of Khari Boli which functions
as the interlanguage for its dialect speakers, i.e., as an associate mother tongue.
Even in the narrowest sense, Hindi is not free of its institutionalized language
styles such as High Hindi (based on the tatsam lexical resources of Sanskrit language),
Cultivated Urdu based on the tatsam Perso-Arabic Lexical resources), and Hindustani
which is based on the tadbhav native lexical resources. The situation can be illustrated
schematically as follows:
PASTE
THE SCANNED IMAGE
In
the above schema:
ABX=Hindustani with the base of Hindi (a variety which is
acquired and not learnt);
ABC=Hindi with ACX as the cultivated, learned variety;
ABD=Urdu with BDX as the cultivated, learned variety;
and
ABCD=The total
range of Hindi, provided one considers Urdu to be linguistically and stylistically
a variant of Hindi.
In the context of the foregoing, it is interesting to note what Chomsky has to
say on the notion of Language:
In
fact the notion Language night turn out just to be a useless notion. For example,
if we fix a certain level of acceptability, then this internally represented system
of grammar generates one set, and we say O.K. that is the language. If we fix
the level a bit differently, the same grammar generates a different set, and we
can say that this is the language. There is no meaningful answer to the question:
Which is the real Language? (Chomsky in Huybregts and Riemsdijk, 1982 : 107).
The second shift of focus, from rule-systems to systems of principles, required
the notion of 'Core grammar' and the concept of extensions and modifications of
such a grammar. In the initial phase of his transformational generative model
Chomsky defined Universal Grammar in terms of systems of rules wherein the system
was a reflection of the biological endowments of the human mind. These biological
endowments were regarded, then, as a function that maps a body of experience into
a particular grammar: the function itself was commonly called 'Universal Grammar'.
In the latest phase of the development of Chomsky's theory, Universal Grammar
is said to be expressed in part, as a system of principles known as 'Core Grammar'
and, in part, as a theory of permissible extensions and modifications of the Core
Grammar.
According to Chomsky, the Core Grammar is that component of the human mind-brain
which is genetically determined in its initial configuration, and this may be
called the initial state of language faculty. It is distinctly marked for two
parts of theory - (a) a theory of principles and parameters, and (b) a theory
of markedness. The latter makes it possible to incorporate more of the idiosyncratic
elements. Although idiosyncratic elements themselves are controlled by innate
principles of the grammar, it is through an interaction between the theory of
principles and the theory of markedness that the initial state of language faculty
is transformed into a more or less steady state leading to the notion of full
grammar.
This second shift of focus from rule systems to systems of principles, as a consequence,
makes most of the results of mathematical linguistics virtually empty. "The
same is true of the mathematical theory of learnability in so far as its results
depend on the presumed infinity of the class of grammars" (Chomsky, 1981b).
Whatever be the focus in a universal Grammar, and whatever shifts might have taken
place, one thing that has remained constant is the fact that theoretical grammars
deal centrally with the structure of language. As a matter of fact, the role of
a theoretical linguistic grammar may be viewed as being two-fold: to privide a
theory of the structure of language per se and to provide the description of the
structure of a language. While the centrality of the question of structure has
always been recognised, the related and more crucial question of the exact locus
of 'structure' has been answered differently by different linguists. Where does
the structure of Language really exist - in the Linguists' mind, in language (corpus)
itself or in language faculty, i.e., in the linguistic consciousness of the language-user,
or some where else?
There are al least three distinct approaches to Universal Grammar which differ
primarily due to the fact that they assign different 'locus' to Language-structure.
According to the Firthian view the structure of a language is merely the analyists'
construct in order to systematise the data. Firth held that "there are no
facts in linguistics until the linguist has made them; they are ultimately, like
all scientific facts, the products of imagination and invention". It is for
this reason that "Linguistics assumes no categories in rebus, no system inherent
in the material awaiting discovery" (Quote from Allen, 1957). The locus of
structure, according to this approach, is the linguist, and it yields General
Grammar (GG) which is the product of invention by linguists in the form of constructs
and categories.
Quite opposite to the above is the view held by Hockett who observes "his
(linguist's) purpose in analysing a language is not to create structure, but to
determine the structure actually created by the speakers of the language. For
the scientist, then, linguistic structure refers to something existing quite independently
of the activities of the analyst; a language is what it is, it has the structure
it has whether studied and analysed by the linguist or not" (Hockett, 1984).
According to this view point the locus of structure is the corpus and, hence,
the linguist is concerned with discovering what is already present in the corpus
itself. This approach yields Discovery Grammar (DG) which is the result of the
discovery of structure via inductive generalizations "which tend to hold
true in more than a chance number of comparisons (such as symmetry and sound system)
or which state tendencies to approach statistical limits across languages or in
one language across time" (Greenberg, Osgood and Jenkins, 1963 : XVIII).
Chomsky has an altogether different notion about the locus of structure. According
to him whatever deep-seated structure a language attests is a function of intrinsic
intellectual organization of a speaker. It is therefore definable only in the
context of universal cognitive traits which produce data and help us to understand
the structure and system that lies deep behind the corpus. According to Chomsky
(1965) "certain highly abstract structures and highly specific principles
of organization are characteristic of all human languages, are intrinsic rather
than acquired, play a central role in perception, as well as, in production of
sentences, and provide the basis for the creative aspect of language use".
Thus, theoretical Universal Grammar, in Chomsky's view, contains the linguistic
rules which are structure-dependent, where a structure itself can be construed
only in terms of innate property of the human mind. The Chomskian approach yields
Innate Grammar (IG) which is the system of principles, conditions and rules that
are elements of all human languages as a necessary structure or innate property
of the human mind, which is the locus of structure.
Linguistic grammars such as DG and IG make assertions of putative language universals
which, by their very nature, are summary statements of those basic characteristics
which are shared by all human beings. Both propose two types of universals - one
in respect of logical structure (i.e., formal), and the other in respect of content
(i.e., substantive), and both of them implicate a theory of language learning.
Contrary to this, Firthian GG explicitly rejects the validity of universal grammar
and any specific theory of learning. According to Firth "Every analysis of
a particular language must of necessity determine the value of ad hoc categories
to which traditional names are given" (Firth, 1957 : 21).
Descriptions of any language based on different models not only differ in their
emphasis on different aspects of language, but even diverge drastically in form
and function or goal and orientation. Secondly, these models with different goals
and orientation and with descriptions of a language in different form and presentation,
make different claims for Learning Theory. For example, transformational model
of a grammar as conceived by Chomsky entails a learning theory based on cognitive
psychology. Basic premises of this theory are in direct conflict with those of
classical behaviourist approaches - i.e., Paviov's classical conditioning theory,
Hulls' habit strength theory, Skinners' instrumental-motivation theory (which
have hitherto been the foundation-stone for structuralists). According to Chomsky:
'Learning theory has, for the most part, concentrated on what seems a much more
marginal topic, namely, the question of species-indedependent regularities in
acquisition of items of a "behavioural repertoire" under experimentally
manipulabel conditions' (Chomsky, 1965 : 56-7).
The most diverging aspects of the two Learning Theories (LT) can be summed up
as below:
Behaviourist
Approach | Cognitive
Approach |
1.a.
Form and function of learning are basically alike across organism (O) and domain(D),
i.e., (O) and (D) in L T (O,D) are not substantial controlling variables. | 1.a.
Form and function of language learning is species-specific and hence, (O) should
be defined as human (H) and (D) as language (L), i.e., L T (O,D) should be conceived
for L T (H,L). |
b.
Language is considered as a 'biosocial' act and hence, this approach sees language
as a resultant of individual behaviour and human culture in general. | b.
Language is considered as 'strictly within biologically determined cognitive capacity
and hence, this approach stipulates UG "as the system of principles, conditions,
and rules that are elements or properties of all human language not merely by
accident but by necessity (Chomsky, 1976 : 29). |
2.
Mechanism of learning is described in terms of conditioning and association nets.
| 2.
Mechanism of learning is described in terms of achievements in cognitive structure
where the role of experience is only to cause the innate schematism to be activated"
(Chomsky, 1971 : 127). |
3.
Learning is evaluated through achievements in overt behavioural acts (i.e., Verbal
performance). | 3.
Learning is evaluated through the growth of internal (linguistic) competence. |
4.
Learning is environment and situation oriented, i.e., principle of discrimination
learning is upheld. | 4.
Learning is creative with the learners' hypothesis formulation ability and testing,
i.e., principle of hypothesis testing is upheld. |
5.
Learning involves primarily an act of inductive generalization. |
5. Learning involves primarily an act of creative productivity. |
The
three types of grammar that we have been under the general category of theoretical
grammars which study language for language's sake, deal with the question: "What
is language", and are concerned with universals across languages. Despite
what has been said earlier about GG, it is as much theory-oriented as IG and DG
are.
It
is worth mentioning at this point, that somehow all theoretical grammar deal primarily
with an idealized conception of language. This led Chomsky to make a distinction
between tow aspects of grammar viz., competence and performance. Competence grammar
is the system of rules which the native speaker has internalized so that he is
able to produce and understand an infinite number of sentences. It is though this
competence grammar that he is able to recognize grammatical mistakes and ambiguities.
This competence grammar is based on an idealized conception of language which
is opposed to the notion of performance, i.e., a specific speech utterance. Chomsky
has been insistent in holding that linguistic before him have been mainly concerned
with specific utterances of actual speech (performance in a corpus) instead of
dealing with competence grammar.
While
the distinction between competence and performance has been widely accepted, literature
on the subject ahs become increasingly critical of or doubtful about the sharpness
of the boundaries between these tow concepts. Speech features such as intonation
pose a special problem in as much as it is difficult to decide whether to assign
them to competence or to performance. As far as performance is concerned, one
has to distinguish between two aspects: (a) those elements which are totally irrelevant
to the abstract rule-system, such as hesitation, pauses, etc., arising out of
lapses of memory or biological limitations of a speaker, or due to socio-psychological
factors involved in speech production - all these elements have to be disregarded
in writing the theoretical grammar of a given language;(b) the use of the tacit
knowledge of language structure in different socio-cultural contexts. The notion
of Communicative Competence as developed by Dell Hymes (1970) focusses on the
native speaker's ability to produce and understand sentences which are appropriate
to the socio-cultural context in which they occur. According to him the term 'competence'
refers to the general capabilities of a person, and is dependent upon both a tacit
knowledge, i.e., competence grammar and the ability for its use, i.e., performance
grammar.
Hymes'
proposed notion of Communicative Competence is, in fact, symptomatic of a third
shift of focus, i.e., a shift from grammar to pragmatics. This shift can be seen
in the works of Gordon and Lakoff (1971), Searle (1975), Grice (1975), etc. This
shift came about as a reaction against the view that accepts form or structure
as a primary in itself, and tends to depreciate use and function as far as linguistic
theory is concerned. it is to be noted here that recent researches in child language
acquisition demonstrate that children are born with a communicative capacity which
gets elaboration and differentiation both before and after a child makes use of
verbal communication (Bullowa, 1979; Bullowa, Jones and Bever, 1964; Bateson,
1971; etc.). Chomsky talks about innate language capacity and language acquisition
device. In hisview the role of experience is only to cause an activation of the
innate schematism and its specified manifestations in a particular manner. The
new-born was never looked upon as having work by Bullowa and Bateson clearly demonstrates
that the child from its very birth behaves as a social communicative being, and
that much before 'proto-conversation'.
The
above shift of focus implies that in linguistic studies the central issue is function
and communication rather than form and structure. We would like to emphasize here
that a Communicative Grammar (based on Communicative competence) is not merely
an extension of a linguistic grammar that includes in its ambit the additional
component of performance grammar; it rather involves a change in theoretical perspective
on language and grammar. Since the difference between linguistic and communicative
grammar is based on two different kinds of orientation it is desirable that we
grasp those salient points which diametrically oppose each other in the two types
of grammar. We summarise below these differences in a tabular form:
Linguistic
Grammar | Communicative
Grammar |
1.
Object of inquiry is language faculty as an element of cognitive capacity. | 1.
Object of inquiry is the functional potential of language in the act of communication |
2.
Perspective of inquiry treats language as verbal semiotic and structure-dependent
semantics. |
2. Perspective of inquiry treats language as a social semiotic and use-dependent
communicative intention. |
3.
context of investigation is monologue. | 3.
Context of investigation is dialogue. |
4.
Modality of investigation is replication of uniformity. | 4.
Modality of investigation is organization of diversity. |
5.
Unit of analysis sentence. |
5. Unit of analysis-speech event/discourse. |
6.
Nature of analysis is rule-oriented. |
6. Nature of analysis is role-oriented. |
7.
Emphasizes form. |
7. Emphasizes function. |
A theoretical grammar centres round the notion of what Chomsky would call 'Knowledge
of Language'. The knowledge of language can be properly characterized by means
of a system of rules that assigns structural descriptions to linguistic expressions.
Thus, knowledge of language gets tied up with the notions of grammar, knowledge
of grammar and the acquisition of grammar. The communicative orientation to the
knowledge of language adds one more component to it, viz., how the knowledge of
grammar is put to use. However, all these notions are restricted to knowledge
of language, and do not have reference to teaching/learning of the ability which
creates in a person this knowledge of language. The creation of the ability to
encode and decode linguistic messages is the concern of Pedagogical Grammar (PG).
It is in this context that Sol Saporta makes a distinction between a scientific
grammar (i.e., a theoretical grammar) and a peadagogical grammar. He says, "A
scientific grammar enumerates the grammatical sentences of a language and provides
each with a structural description and a semantic interpretation. The pedagogical
grammar ideally attempts to develop the native speaker's ability to recognize
and produce sentences. That is, "a speaker can accept an arbitrarily selected
sequence of elements in his vocabulary, determine whether or not it is a sentence
in his language and, if so, assign to it its correct structural description and
semantic interpretation" (Sol Saporta, 1966).
While it is true that the three types of grammar are related to one another, there
is an essential difference in their goals, as well as, in their intrinsic properties.
The accountability of a universal grammar is centred round a theory of language,
while that of a language-specific descriptive grammar is centered round the corpus.
As distinct from these two, a pedagogical grammar has accountability in terms
of utility, communicability and teachability/learnability. A universal grammar
may be termed theory-oriented (i.e., universals across languages), a language-specific
grammar as corpus (text)-oriented (i.e., generalised statements across data),
and a pedagogical grammar as learner-oriented (i.e., based on learning strategies
and learner behaviour).
chart
Certain facts about pedagogical grammars may be mentioned at this juncture. A
pedagogical grammar falls within the domain of applied linguistics. It may be
pointed out here that applied linguistics is a field of activity wherein the aims
of studying languages go beyond the study of language for its won sake. This implies
a clear distinction between theoretical linguistics and applied linguistics -
a theoretical linguist is thus a creator of theories about language, while the
role of the applied linguist is that of a consumer of theories (Allen, 1973 :
ix). However, we should also remember that applied linguists are those who exploit
the gains of linguistics knowledge, keeping in view the requirements of those
who are in need of such knowledge. Seen in this perspective, applied linguistics
is not merely linguistics or the use of linguistics any more than medicine is
chemistry or, as Bolinger (1968) has said, "the use of chemistry".
It is in this light that the new approach to second language/Foreign language
teaching shifts the focus of attention from the formal grammatical to the functional,
communicative characteristics of a language. Language teaching is now being oriented
to enable the students to use the language for special purposes. Thus, now instead
of talking about the teaching of a language in general terms, one witnesses the
teaching of contextually-defined language variants (LSP, i.e., language for Special
Purposes). Courses in LSP are devised on the premise that different elements of
language structure are sensitive to different kinds of situational features and,
thus, language structures which remain the same on the deep level, get realized
differently in form and texture on the surface level.
Being applied in nature, the form and functions of pedagogical grammars differ
because of certain variables of application. The following three variables are
worth mentioning:
1)
Teacher/Learner Variable
There orientation of a pedagogical grammar differs
depending upon whether it keeps at its centre the teacher or the learner. A teacher-oriented
pedagogical grammar takes a detour via contrastive linguistics, while a learner-oriented
pedagogical grammar takes a detour via error-linguistics.
2)
Need/Use Variable
There are different uses of language such as literary
scientific, etc. Earlier it was assumed that the differences between them were
restricted to lexis or to the frequency of occurrence of certain grammatical forms.
However, now it has been established that they represented not different forms
of language but different ways of using the same language. Grammatical structures,
thus, partake different communicative functions and, in the process, get oriented
differently. Based on different use and usages pedagogical grammars also get differently
oriented as far as functionality of grammatical structures, usage of lexical items
and cohesion in textual composition are concerned.
3)
Methodics Variable
This is concerned primarily with the types of dialogue
that exist between teacher and student, between teaching machine and student or
between student and student, and the choice of media such as programmed learning,
audio-lingual materials, exercise books, etc. All of these have a bearing upon
the selection, gradation and presentation of linguistic items in a pedagogical
grammar.
These
three variables can be integrated into a problem-process perspective, yielding
thereby the following schema:
chart
Pedagogical
grammars thus, have a direct relationship with theoretical, as well as, language-specific
descriptive grammars. However, a pedagogical grammar, in addition, includes many
more things. A pedagogical grammar, as stated earlier, is applied in nature, and
exists in its own right. We cannot look at this discipline as merely a sub-set
of linguistics, since linguistics is concerned primarily with the code of a language,
while pedagogical grammar deals with the use of code made maximally effective
in language teaching/learning operation and hence, the nature of its grading is
not always congruent with the ordering of rules in a theoretical or language-specific
grammar.