VERBAL SYSTEM


Tangkhul-Naga Grammar ( A Study of Word Formation ) 

 

Prev | Home | Next

4.0 Introduction

 

In traditional grammar, the verb is sometimes defined notionally as a ‘doing’ word.  Such a notional definition has generally been considered inadequate for a number of reasons.  Modern grammars prefer a more syntactical definition.

 

A more exhaustive definition is that verbs constitute a major word class that is normally essential to clause structure and which inflects and can show contrast of tense-aspect-modality, number, person, and voice.  Verbs tend to code less time-stable experiences, primarily transitory states, events or actions. Verbs may code either extremely rapid changes, or processes that may have a certain duration, or even relatively more stable states.  In other words, they cover a certain range from one extreme end of the time-stability scale.

 

Verbs are also most obviously distinguished by the fact that each verb typically requires the presence in its sentence of a specified set of NP arguments, each of which may be required to appear in some particular grammatical form (particular case marking, particular pre/post-position, etc.).

 

4.0.1. Adjectives as constituting a sub-class of verb

 

In Tangkhul-Naga, structurally, there is no distinction between adjectives (modifiers) and verbs (predicators).  All (verbal) roots can function, with appropriate affixation, as noun modifiers or predicators. All (verbal) roots can have all the grammatical inflections, and can well denote states, events or actions.  In other words, a word which is an adjective in some other languages is a verb (or a sub-class of verb) in Tangkhul-Naga.  Thus, for example, the following stems, which are not very different semantically from one another, basically function as verbals.

 

yúi- šì

‘(to) envy/be jealous’ (<stare-sulk)

y∂̀ η- šì

‘(to) be hostile’ (<look-bad)

rì η- šì

‘(to) be poor’ (<live-bad)

 

The phenomena in Tangkhul-Naga (and many other languages) support the opinion of some linguists that verbs and adjectives can be grouped together into a class of ‘function’ words of ‘predicates’ and can be differentiated rather sharply from nouns.  Furthermore, the fact that ‘adjectives’ are inherently relational in nature and are therefore able to function as predicates more readily than nouns, and than they are similar to verbs in denoting the characteristics of objects’, 12 are seen as supporting the claim.

12 D.N.S. Bhat, op cit p. 43

 

The characteristics that are to be used in support of the claim that verbs and adjectives form a single category in Tangkhul-Naga include :

 

(1)   the occurrence of the same type of inflectional affixe in the predicative use; and

(2)   the occurrence of hte same type of nominalizing, adjectivalizing and adverbializing processes for referential and modifying uses.

 

This, in spite of having some minor structural differences, such as, some types of roots (but not verbal roots) occurring with certain types of partially reduplicated intensifiers/modifiers, adjectives will summarily be considered as constituting a sub-type of verb in this discussion.

 

4.0.2. Verbs in Tangkhul-Naga

 

The major class of words designated as verbs on the basis of their mopho-syntactic characteristics is very rich in Tangkhul-Naga, particularly, from the point of ‘compounding’, ‘serial constructions’ and inflectional systems, involving distinction of tense-aspect-modality.  No gender and number distinctions are made.  The language favor active construction and does not make much differences between passive and active construction as there is no passive ‘verbal morphology’, that is, the passive is not marked in the morphology of the verb.

 

The criteria for designating the class of words as berbs in Tangkhul-Naga are :

 

(1)   syntactically occuring as head of a predicate ;

(2)   accepting tense-aspect –modality markers;

(3)   accepting non-finite marker/nominalizer /k∂̀/ ~ /kh∂̀/

 

4.1. Types of verbal roots/stems

A simpleroot is a monomorphemic irreducible ‘core element/semanteme’ obtained by dropping all the affixes.  A root may be monosyllabic or bisyllabic.  Examples :

 

niη

‘be’

c∂̀t

‘walk’

lèi

‘exist/have’

‘go’

‘come back’

òn

‘change’

‘come’

m∂̀lái

‘forget’

 

4.1.2. Complex /derived verbal stems

 

Many Tangkhul-Naga verbal stems, which are bound forms, are derived from roots or expressives.  Derivation is a derived from roots or expressives.  Derivation is a productive process in the language.  Often verbs are derived from ‘nominal’ roots, other’ verbal’ roots or expressives.  There are three main derivative prefixes - - / η∂̀-/, /ph∂̀-/ and /-k∂̀-/.  For example consider the following verbal stems derived from (1) roots and (2) expressives.

 

 

 

(1)   Derivation from roots

 

A number of complex verbal stems are derived from roots.  The derived form is a bound form and cannot occur in isolation.  The following are some examples of contrasting derivation of nouns and verbs from roots :

 

Root

Derived verb stem

Derived noun stem

ph∂̀-rǜ

‘to lay egg’

à-r ǘ

‘egg’

na

η∂̀-nà

‘to listen’

kh∂̀-nà

‘ear’

 

(2)   Derivation from expressives

 

A number of complex verbal stems (which are bound forms) are derived from expressives by prefixing /k∂̀/.  Thus, consider the following forms :

 

Expressive

 

Derived verb root

tuη tuη

‘thumbing sound’

k∂̀-túη

‘(to) produce thumbing sound’

tap tap

‘splashing sound’

k∂̀-tap

‘(to) splash’

 

4.1.3. Compounding of roots

 

There are two main types of compound verb roots in the language - - (1) compounding of noun root and verb root (N+V) and (2) compounding of two verb roots (V+V)

 

4.1.3.1. N+V Compound root

 

In this type of compounds two roots (one noun and the other verbal) are combined to form a single verbal root expressing a ‘single’ verbal idea.  Some of the compounds thus formed are historically complex that the meaning of a root of the compound is lost to the present-day speakers of the language.  The unidentifiable item (marked with ‘?’) is always a root by the fact that the compound root allows infixation in derivation and inflection, which is typical characteristic of all verbal compounds in the language.  Again, the first root/constituent is   a noun by virtue of its inability to take verbal inflection in isolation. There are a fairly large number of N+V compound in the language.  The following examples shows how a noun root can be combined with a number of verbal roots to form compound verb roots.

niη- šì

‘(to) thank’

(< mind-be pensive)

niη-càη

‘(to) desire’

(< mind-tire)

niη-s∂η

‘(to) concentrate/be attentive’

(<mind-put)

rü-c∂̀ η

‘(to) be thirsty’

(< liquid-THIRST)

rü-hìk

‘(to) baptize’

(< liquid-submerge and emerge)

rü-yak

‘(to) swin’

(< liquid-climb, descend or cross with hands clinging on to something for main support (as in climbing well))

rü-vái

‘(to) take bath’

(< liquid-wear)

khón- šì

‘(to) curse’

(< sound-bad)

lèi- šì

‘(to) love’

(<?-mind/sulk)

là η-so

‘(to) be proud’

(<?-praise)

 

4.1.3.2. Idiomatic VV compounds

 

Tangkhul-Naga has a rich number of verbs formed by compounding (or conjoining) two verbs to express a single verbal idea.  In most cases such compounds are ‘idiomatic’ in the sense that the meanings of the two verbs in isolation are not directly relatable to their ‘combined’ gloss.  The following are some example of idiomatic VV compounds :

 

ph∂̀niη-úη

šìt-s∂η

think-come = ‘remember’

count-put ‘trust/believe’

rìη-phá

rìη- šì

live-good ‘be happy’

live-bad ‘be poor’

y∂̀η- šì

yúi- šì

look-bad ‘be hostile’

stare-bad ‘envy’

r ǘ-y∂̀η

ph∂̀niη-và

pass through-look ‘peep’

think-go ‘imagine’

η∂̀hán-ká

 

ask-go on the other side ‘answer’

 

 

4.1.4. Collocationally restricted verbs

 

There are a fairly large number of verbs having collocational restrictions with nouns.  They can be subgrouped into two types - - (1) fixed collocation and (2) free collocation.

 

4.1.4.1. Fixed collocation

A verb having collocational restriction is fixed in the sense that the particular verb can co-occur with only one particular nominal.  Thus, the verb roots/stems are meaningless in isolation, that is, if not introduced by the nominal stems they collocate with.  The following are some of the verbs of this type :

 

k∂̀-ziη ró

‘(to) rain’

(<sky-RAIN)

k∂̀- ziη hàm

‘(to) sunshine’

(<sky-UNSHINE)

niη m∂̀oη

‘(to) doubt’

(<sky-DOUBT)

kh∂̀-nà  η∂̀khok

‘(to) be deaf’

(<sky-DEAF)

mìk η∂̀peo

‘(to) be blind’

(<sky-BLIND)

kh∂̀k- η∂̀sü

‘(to) be dumb’

(<sky-DUMB)

mai η∂̀tap

‘(to) wash face’

(<sky-WASH)

kúi kh∂̀yào

‘(to) wash head’

(<sky-WASH)

pàη m∂̀cìk

‘(to) wash hand’

(<sky-WASH)

4.1.4.2. Free collocation

 

Verbs having free collocation are those which can co-occur with more than one noun.  Verbs of this type express specialized or metaphorical meanings.  Thus, often their meanings in isolation are not directly relatable to those in collocated structures.  The following are some of the most commonly used such verbs :

 

m∂̀lùη vat

‘(to) be angry’

(<heart-burst)

m∂̀lùη ním

‘(to) be sober’

(<heart-cool)

kh∂̀yá šì

‘(to) respect’

(<prestige-mind)

ùk  kh∂̀n∂η

‘(to) be sad’

(<soul chamber-tire)

m∂ηlà tùk

‘(to) have nightmare’

(<soul-arrest)

k∂̀ziη rái

‘(to) stop raining’

(<sky-recover (as from sickness))

k∂̀ziη šì

‘(to) be cloudy’

(<sky-sulk)

rìkhan η∂̀šéi

‘(to) strike lightning’

(<lightning-shake)

k∂̀ziη η∂̀šùm

‘(to) thunder’

(<regret and wail)

làηpar tak

‘(to) strike bolt’

(<thunderbold-hit)

η∂̀càη c∂̀t

‘(to) be lazy’

(<stimulus-break)

η∂̀càη ká

‘(to) be industrious’

(<stimulus-attach)

niη teo

‘(to) be narrow-minded’

(<mind-small)

Some verbal stems occur only in the negative forms such as :

à-phar

m -z∂η           

 

Fx-lung

NEG-contain

 

‘(to) be cowardly’ (< lung not containing)

niη

m -piéi

 

mind NEG-full ‘(to) be mentally abnormal’ (<mind not full)

 

4.2. Transitivity

 

The most common derivational morphemes for verbs in the language are those affecting transitivization/causativization.  Basically, Tangkhul-Naga has no productive inbuilt system of deriving transitive or intransitive stems from each other, except causativization (see discussion below).  Quite often, a transitive syntactic structure is assigned to semantically intransitive verbs by way of metaphoric extension of either the prototype ‘agent’ or the prototype ‘patient’.  This tendency in the language is very striking, and is either an indication, a cause or result of a conspicuous feature of the language.  The notion ‘transitive’ is much more syntactic, much less semantic.  Thus, for example, consider the following syntactically transitive verbs :

 

 

(1) i (-n )

n∂-li

théi-y∂̀

 

     I

you-DAT

know-NFUT

‘I know you’

(2) i (-n )

n∂-li

m∂̀lùη

vat-∂̀

      I (-NOM)

you-DAT

heart

burst-NFUT

(3) i (-n )

n∂-li

ph∂̀niη-∂̀

 

      I (-NOM)

you-DAT

think-NFUT

 

‘I think of you’

 

 

Thus, when a less prototypical transitive verb is coded syntactically as a member of the class of the transitive prototype, in some sense the user of the language construes its properties as being similar, analogical, reminiscent of the prototype.  In other word, we have here the most common linguistic phenomenon of metaphoric extension.

 

4.2.1.      Transitivization

 

In very few cases there are separate roots for parallel sets of transitive and intansitive verbs.  Examples :

Intransitive

Transitive

cui

‘(to) burn’

sǘ

‘(to) burn’

hòr

‘(to) shine’

šàr

‘(to) light’

 

However, such sets are very rare, and there are a number of verbs transitivized  through internal changes in the roots or by prefixation.  (Instances of such verbs are, again, very limited, and thus, no derivational rule can be predicted).  Examples are :

 

m∂̀thùk

‘be awake’

m∂̀thot

‘wake (somebody up)’

η∂̀cì

‘be afraid’

m∂̀cìt

‘threaten’

z∂η

‘contain’

š∂ η

‘put (in)’

pàk

‘be wide’

phak

‘widen’

η∂̀nùk

‘shake’

kh∂̀nùk

‘shake’

 

4.2.2.      Morpho-lexical causativization

 

All causative operators in Tangkhul-Naga are morphological, that is, all manipulative verbs are co-lexicalized with their complements.  The verb-affix status of the causative morphemes is nothing but the diachronic consequence of co-lexicalization of its precursor - -  a manipulative causative main verb - -  with its complement verb-stem.  Over time, such a process usually gravitates towards a lexical ‘derivational’ process, whereby the erstwhile verb (‘make’, ‘cause’ loses its independent lexical status, undergoes morpho-phonological reduction (bleaching’), and becomes a causative affix on the verb.  At that stage, one may still consider it an inflectional morpheme marking the syntactic process of transitivization.  But the potential for considering it derivational is just as strong.  Indeed, over time the process become less regular and more lexically-governed.

 

There are three causative morphemes in Tangkhul-Naga /cì/~/ šì/, (2) /sá/ and (3) /η∂̀s k/.  They are discussed below in turn.

 

 

 

4.2.2.1. Causative /cì/~/ šì/

 

Causative /cì/~/ šì/ are allomorphs of the causative morpheme which is prefixed to a number of transitive and intransitive verbal roots to derive transitive verbs.  This morpheme codes causation with an agentive/non-agentive human manipulee.  In the process of causativization, the verbal roots may be diverged phonologically.  /cì/  occurs before obstruents and / šì/ occurs before sonorants.  Examples :

 

Root

 

Causativized stem

 

théi

‘see’

cì- théi

‘show’

šà

‘hear’

cì- šà

‘announce’

šùk

‘borrow’

cì- šùk

‘lend’ (especially money)

‘go up’

cì- ká

‘send up’

šok

‘go out’

cì- šok

‘send out’

z∂η

‘go in’

cì- z∂η

‘send in’

‘come’

cì- rá

‘send towards’

‘go’

cì- và

‘send away’

m∂̀η

‘drink’

šì- m∂̀η

‘feed (liquid)’

maη

‘be lost’

šì- man

‘lose’

Some verbs are derived with / šì/ from expressives and nouns. 

Thus, consider the following example :

šì-met

‘press with fingers; (muscles, fruits, etc.)

from :

 

 

 

mét met

‘feeling of pressing some soft solid objects (such as muscles, etc.)

 

4.2.2.2       Causativizer/transitivizer /sa/

 

The verb sá ‘do’, ‘make’ is prefixed to a few number of intransitive roots to derive transitive verbal stems.  This morpheme codes causation with an animate-agentive manipulee.  In the process of causativization, the root may be diverged phonologically. Examples are :

 

Intransitive root

 

Transitive stem

 

thì

‘die’

sà-th∂̀t

‘kill’

kai

‘break (solid object)’

sá-khai

‘break’

kak

‘break (sheet etc.)’

sá-khak

‘tear’

c∂̀t

‘break (threat, etc.)’

sá- š∂̀t

‘break/cut’

ùt

‘get holed’

sá-phùt

‘make hole’

zǘ

‘be touched

sá- zǘ

‘touch’

 

4.2.2.3    Causative /η∂̀s k/

 

The most productive morphological device of causativization in the language is effected by suffixation of the causative /η∂̀s k/ ‘to cause’, ‘to allow’, ‘to permit’, ‘to let’.  It can be added to any verbal root/stem (instransitive, transitive or causativized).  This causative morpheme codes causation with both animate and inanimate manipulee.  Examples :

 

Root

 

Causative stem

 

c∂̀p

‘cry’

c∂̀p- η∂̀s k

‘cause to cry’

‘give’

mì- η∂̀s k

‘cause to give’

zìk

‘be black’

zìk- η∂̀s k

‘blacken’, cause to become black’

 

4.2.2.4    Compounding of causative morphemes

 

A complex situation is formed when the causative morphemes are combined to code causation.  Thus, consider the following sentences :

(1) Àton

c∂̀p-∂̀

 

       A.

cry-NFUT

‘Aton cried’ (intransitive)

(2) Àton-li

cì-cat-∂̀

 

       A/

-DAT CAUS-cry-NFUT

 

‘(Somebody) made Aton cry (by not attending her)’

(3) Àton-li

c∂̀p- η∂̀s k-∂̀

 

      A. -DAT

cry-DAUS-NFUT

 

‘(Somebody) made Aton cry’ (AGT/Non-AGT manipulee)

(4) Àton-li

sá-cì-cat-∂̀

 

      A. -DAT

do-CAUS-cry-NFUT

(AGT manipulee)

‘(Somebody) made Aton cry’ (by pinching here)

(5) Àton-li

sá-cì-cat- η∂̀s k-∂̀

 

      A.-DAT

do-CAUS-cry-CAUS-NFUT

 

     ‘(Somebody) made someone makes Aton cry/

      (Somebody) made Aton makes someone cry’

 

Sentence (5) above in ambiguous because of non-indicationof the direct and indirect manipulee(in order). Thus, compare (6) below:

 

(6) (a)  Jóhn-n∂̀

Peter-li

Àton-li

sá-cì-cat- η∂̀s k-∂̀

            J. –NOM

P. –DAT

A. –DAT

do-CAUS-cry-CAUS-NFUT

          ‘John had Peter makes Aton cry’

      (b) Jóhn-n∂̀

Àton-li

Peter-li

sá-cì-cat- η∂̀s k-∂̀

           ‘J. –NOM

A. –DAT

P. –DAT

do-CAUS-cry-CAUS-NFUT

           ‘John had Aton makes PEter cry’

 

4.2.3. Reciprocal /η∂̀ro k/

 

The most general syntactic definition or reciprocals is that two like events are at issue, with subject of the first being the object of the second, and vice versa.  The two participants thus act upon each other (reciprocally).  Such a definition needs to be expanded because in Tangkhul-Naga (as may be in many other languages, too), reciprocal constructions may also express states, actions or arrangements involving two or more people of groups of people sharing the same feeling as someone else or behaving in the same way agreeing to help ‘each other’/ ‘one another’, and so on.  This is illustrated in the following examples.  The root /η∂̀s k/, ‘to go and meet someone coming from the opposite direction’, is suffixed to another root for constructing varied types of reciprocals in the language.

(1)  i-ní

šào- η∂̀rok-∂̀

      l-DU

beat-RECI-NFUT

     ‘We (two) beat each other’

(2)  à-ní

šì- η∂̀rok-∂̀

      3-DU

sulk-RECI-NUT

     ‘They (both) sulked’ (Feeling bad about each other)

(3)  i-ní

 

       l-DU

chase-RECI-NFUT

      ‘One of us chase the other’

      (Lit: We (two) chased each other)

(4) i-ní

yòη- η∂̀rok-∂̀

      1-DU

chase-RECI-NFUT

     ‘We (two) ran (to see who is faster)’

(5)  thui-kh∂̀rè-pa-n∂̀

m∂̀thot- η∂̀rok-∂̀

      get up-first-SPEC-NOM

wake-RECI-FUT

     ‘The one who gets up earlier will wake up the other’

(6) k cí-cá-wùi

ot

sá-η∂̀rok-s∂̀      

      each-REDU-GEN

work

do-RECI-HOR

‘Let each of us do one’s own work’ (No interference in others’ business) (Lit:?   Let’s do work of each.)

 

 

Apart from the above discussed type of reciprocals, there is a distinct group of transitive verbs that are inherently reciprocal.  Such verbs may be expressed with ‘conjoined subjects’ and carry a reciprocal meaning without any reciprocal marking on the verb.  Verbs of this type in the language are illustrated below :

                                   

(1) (a)

à-ní

η∂̀mǘ-∂̀

 

 

3-DU

fight-NFUT

‘They (two) fought’

     (b)

à-ní

η∂̀mǘ-η∂̀rok-∂̀

 

 

3-DU

fight-RECI-NFUT

‘They (two) fought each other’

(2) (a)

à-ní

η∂̀ph∂t-∂̀

 

 

3-DU

argue-NFUT

‘They (two) argued’

     (b)

à-ní

η∂̀ph∂t́-η∂̀rok -∂̀

 

 

3-DU

argue-RECI-NFUT

‘They (two) argued each other’

 

The semantic difference between the two patterns may be that the reciprocals with no distinct morphology are more likely to be interpreted as a fully integrated ‘single event’, while the one with reciprocal marker may be interpreted, at least in some contexts, as a succession of separate events.  However, at least for some verbs, the distinction may not be clear.

4.3 Tense-aspect-modality

 

In this section we will consider the morpho-semantic and ‘discourse-pragmatic’ features clustering the various categories comprising the complex system of tense-aspect-modality.  As morphological features, they tend to cluster around the verb.  As semantic features, they are intimately involved in the meaning-structure of verbs (‘predicates’).  They code various facets of the state, event of action.  And as discourse-pragmatic features, they play a crucial role in the sequencing of propositions in discourse, in ‘foregrounding’ or ‘backgrounding’ them, and in indicating their time truth/certainty/probability modalities vis-à-vis the speaker-hearer contact.

 

4.3.1. Tense

 

One may distinguish two tenses in Tangkhul-Naga  - - (1) Non-future and (2) Future.  The relation between the two may be represented diagrammatically as follows :

 

Tense and time :

 

Non-future                   Future

 

 

 

                                         Speech time

4.3.1.1. Non-Future

Non-future tense is marked by the suffix /∂̀/.  It has three phonologically conditioned allomorphs - - /y∂̀/. /i/. /w∂̀/. Non-future tense codes as event or state whose event-time or occurrence-time preceded, or is right at, the time of speech.  Thus, the suffix /∂̀/ indicates either the past or the present tense.  With stative verbs, it may also signal habitual aspect.  Thus, consider the following.

 

(1) n∂

théi-∂̀

 

      you

know-NFUT

‘You know/knew’

(2) n∂

kào-w∂̀

 

      you

thin-NFUT/HAB

‘You are thin’

 

4.3.1.2. Future

 

The future tense in the language is marked by the suffix /r∂̀/. Example :

 

i

và-r

 

I

go-FUT

‘I’ ll go’

 

 

 

4.3.2.      Aspect

 

Aspect in Tangkhul-Naga, as elsewhere, encompasses a group of heterogeneous semantic and pragmatic categories.  Some involve temporal properties such as progressive, inceptive or perfective.  Others involve purely pragmatic notions such as relevance or immediacy.

 

In this study the aspectual system of the language is broadly divided into three categories (1) progressive/durative, (2) punctual and (3) perfect.

 

4.3.2.1.Progressive/durative

 

The progressive aspect construes an event as having no initial or terminal boundaries.  Its use implies that, from the perspective of the speaker, an event or a state is described in the middle of happening or existence, with its boundaries disregarded and its temporal span accentuated.  The progressive aspectuals are divided into the following sub-categories.

 

4.3.2.1.1.      Present progressive/durative /lì/, /-t∂ lèi - y∂̀/

 

There are two present progressive markers in the language - - (1) /lì/  and (2) a compound /-t∂ lèi-y∂̀/ (where /t∂/ = the adverbializer/conjunctive particle.  lèi = ‘be’, ‘exist’, ‘have’, and /y∂̀/= Non Future). li is preferably used only in non-declarative sentences.  The following are some examples of present progressive.

 

(1)  (a)  n∂

khì

sá-lì

 

             you

what

do-PPROG

‘What are you doing’

       (b) i

ph∂zà-t∂

lèi-y∂̀

 

            ‘I’m

eating’

(Lit : I exist eating)

 

(2)      (ó?)

n∂-v∂̀

pí-lùi-lì

 

         ‘(Oh!)

you-TOP

sleep-AGAIN-PPROG

‘(Oh!) You-re sleeping agin’

(3)       i

pí-lì

 

 

            I

sleep-PPROG

 

‘I’ m sleeping’

 

 

4.3.2.1.2.      Past/future progressive /sa/

 

Past and future progressive aspects are marked by suffixing the delexicalized/grammaticalized verb/sà/ ‘hear’, followed by non-future and future tense markers respectively.  (Henceforth, sa=’hear’ or PROG (ressive) aspect marker).

 

(1)   (a)

Past progressive :

 

 

i pí-ciη-∂̀

 

 

I sleep-HAB-NFUT

‘I always sleep’

        (b)

Future progressive :

 

 

i  pí-sà-r∂̀

 

 

I sleep –PROG-FUT

‘ I will be sleeping’

(2)    (a)

Past progressive :

 

 

i  pí-t∂

lèi-sà-ì

 

 

I sleep-ADV

be-PROG-NFUT

‘I was sleeping’

        (b)

Future progressive

 

 

 

i  pí-t∂

lèi-sà-r

 

 

I sleep-ADV

be-PROG-FUT

‘I will be sleeping’

 

4.3.2.1.3.      Simultaneous progressive /m∂̀núη/

 

The progressive perspective of an event is often established through bringing the observer on to the scene in the middle of the event where it is already going on . Examples are :

(a) Simultaneous present :

      i

pí-m∂̀húη-t∂

lèi-y∂̀

      I

sleep-SIMUL-ADV

be-NFUT

     ‘I’ m in the middle of (my) sleep’

(b) Simultaneous past :

      i

pí-m∂̀húη-sà-ì

 

      I

sleep-SIMUL-PROG-NFUT

     ‘I’ was in the middle of (my) sleep’

(b) Simultaneous future :

      i

pí-m∂̀húη-sà-r

 

      I

sleep-SIMUL-PROG-FUT

     ‘I’ will be in the middle of (my) sleep (when you come)’

 

4.3.2.1.4.      Habitual aspectual /cíη/

/cíη/ expresses a regular or consistent performance or occurrence of an action or a state.  Examples :

(1)  Non-future habitual :

       i

pí-cíη-∂̀

       I

sleep-HAB-NFUT

‘I always sleep

(2)  Future habitual :

       i

pí-cíη-r∂̀

       I

sleep-HAB-FUT

‘I will always sleep

(3)  Continuous :

 (a)  Past :

i    pí-cíη-r∂̀

       I

I   sleep-HAB-PROG-NFUT

‘I was still/always sleeping’

 (b) Present :

i   pí-cíη-t∂̀

lèi-y∂̀

 

I sleep-HAB-ADV

be-NFUT

 

‘I am still sleeping’

 

 (c) Future :

i   pí-cíη-sà-r∂̀

 

 

I sleep-HAB-PROG-FUT

 

 

‘I will be still sleeping/

 

 

I will be sleeping till then’

 

 

4.3.2.1.5.      Reduplication as habitual/iterative action

 

A verb root is reduplicated to signal the habitual/iterative aspect, as in :

(1)  Past :

i

và- và

cì-y∂̀

 

I

go-go

COMP-NFUT

 

‘I used to go’ (Lit : It is that I go go)

(2) Future :

i

và- và

cì-r∂̀

 

I

go-go

COMP-FUT

 

‘I will keep going’ (Lit: Will be that I go go)

 

4.3.2.1.6.      Inceptive/progressive aspectuals

 

The modality verb /phok/ ‘create’ patterns as an inceptive aspectuals marker.  (Henceforth phok= INCEP (tive) aspectuals marker)

 

ákh m∂

sá-phok-r

 

tomorrow

do-INCEP—FUT

‘(I) will start doing tomorrows’

 

4.3.2.2.Immediate aspect /ù/

 

An exhaustively used discourse particle /u/ is used to render an event or a state more urgent, vivid or immediate. This also involves a manipulation of the pragmatic perspective of the discourse, as if the speaker invites the hearer to be present just before the occurrence of an event, and be more emotionally involved.  The immediate aspect contrast with the simple past or future, which code a more ‘remote’ perspective on the event. The usage of the immediate aspect marker is illustrated below with ‘rough’ translations :

 

(1)  á-rúi

và-ù-r

 

       FX-now

go-IMM-FUT

 

       ‘I will go now (without any delay)’

(2)  và-ù-l

 

 

      go-IMM-IMP

‘(You) go (don’t delay further)’

 

4.3.2.3. Punctual aspectual /háo/

/háo/ is a complex punctual/compact/bounded aspect marker.  It construes an event or a state as having vivid initial and less-explicit terminal temporal boundaries.  Like the immediate aspect marker /ù/, the usage of /háo/ also renders the events or states  more immediate.  It involves a manipulation of the pragmatic perspective of the  discourse, as if the narrator invites the hearer to observe a scene from close proximity, taking up the entire event ‘frame’, so that it is viewed as a protracted object with boundaries on both sides.  The punctual perspective may be diagrammatically represented as :

Punctual perspective /háo/ :

 

               time flow                   event

                                            referent time

For interpretation of the above diagram, we may consider the following :

 

(1)          ...... hi

haη-l∂̀k∂

à

thì-háo-w∂

              .......this

say-CP

3SG

die-PUNC-NFUT

              ........saying this, he/she died’

(2) (a)  k -thà-háo-kh l

 

 

 

           WH-LIKE-PUNC-Q

 

 

 

          ‘What happened?’ (Lit:? Has been like what?)

     (b) i-ní

η∂̀ph∂t-l∂̀k∂

à-n∂̀

sào-háo-w∂̀

          I-DU

argue-CP

3SG-NOM

beat-PUNC-NFUT

         ‘We argued and (then) he/she beat me’

 (3)      i

árúi

và-háo- r

 

            I

now

go-PUNC-FUT

 

           ‘I will go now (without delay)’

 

 

4.3.2.4. Perfect aspect /r∂/

 

The perfect is functionally the most complex aspect in Tangkhul-Naga.  It involves a cluster of features, some more semantic, others more pragmatic.  The perfect aspect marker /r∂/ is used in combination with other aspect markers in any tense.  The perfect is sub-divided into the following groups.

 

 

4.3.2.4.1.      Immediate present perfect /ù-r∂̀/

 

The immediate present perfect is marked by combining the immediate aspect marker /ù/ and the perfect aspect marker /r∂/.  This is probably used much more frequently in oral narrative, often indicating the tense, truth, certainty, or probability vis-a-vis the speaker-hearer contact. Thus, consider the following :

(1) à-thùm 

rá-ù-r∂

      3-PL

come-IMM-PERF

‘They have come’ (standing at the door or only few yards away)

(2) thak-lù 

Àton  

thì-ù-r∂̀

      quick-IMP

A.

die-IMM-PERF

‘Quick! Aton has died’ (Or, is she? Might be she has just fainted or . . . . . . . )

 

 

 

4.3.2.4.2.      Present perfect /hái- r∂/

 

Present perfect is marked by combining the grammaticalized verb /hái/ meaning ‘leave/keep’ with the perfect marker /r∂/.  (Henceforth, hái = ‘leave’ or PP (past/passive participle).  Some examples of present perfect are :

 

 

(1) Àton

và-hái-r∂

 

       A.

go-PP-PERF

‘Aton has gone’

(2) Àton

kào-hái-r∂-sà-ì

 

       A.

thin-PP-PERF-PROG-NFUT

 

      Àton had become thin’ (Lit : Heard Aton has become thin)

 

4.3.2.4.3.      Past perfect /hái-r∂/ and progressive /sà/ and non-future /ì/.  Examples :

 

The past perfect is marked by the combination of the present perfect marker/hái-àr∂/ and progressive /sà/ and non-future /ì/.  Examples :

 

4.3.2.4.4.      Future perfect /hái-r∂-sà-r (-lì)/

 

The Future perfect is marked with or without the present progressive /lì/ which expresses preconditioning . Examples :

 

(1)        i

và-hái-r∂-sà-r

            I

go-PP-PERF-PROG-FUT

‘I will have gone’

(2)   thà-∂̀kh

à

và-hái-r∂-sà-r -lì

        so-COND

3SG

go-pp-PERF-PROG-FUT-PPROG

       ‘If so/in that case, he/she will have gone’

        (preconditioning/logical conclusion

 

4.3.2.4.5.      Other aspectuals

There are several other aspectual markers in the language involving the perfect and the progressive depicting events as ongoing or terminated.  One of such aspectual is the use of the grammaticalized verb /rá/ ‘come’ as a progressive/continuative aspect marker, as in:

 

(a)  Present perfect progressive :

       Àton    

(k∂̀-sáη-kh∂̀)

c∂̀p-rá-hái-r∂

      A.

(for a long time)

cry-come-PP-PERF

     ‘Aton has been crying (for a long time)’

(b)  Past perfect progressive

       Àton    

c∂̀p-rá-hái-r∂-sà-ì

 

      A.

cry-come-PP-PERF-PROG-NFUT

     ‘Aton has been crying (for a long time)’

(c)  Future perfect progressive:

       Àton    

c∂̀p-rá-hái-r∂-sà-r

 

      A.

cry-come-PP-PERF-PROG-FUT

     ‘Aton will have been crying

 

 

4.3.3.      Modality

 

Of all the widely attested grammatical categories, modality is perhaps the most elusive. Mood distinctions in Tangkhul-Naga often tend to shade off almost imperceptibly into expression of the speaker’s attitude and into clearly pragmatic factors, such as the speaker’s perceived relationship to other people.

 

Mood in the language is expressed variously with ‘modal auxiliaries’ appended to various finite and non-finite verbal forms, with or without the expression of tense and/or aspect.  A detailed study of modality is beyond the scope of this work, and we will consider only those which are morphologically distinguishable, such as,  imperative, interrogative, hortative, optative, subjunctive, dubitative, permission, obligatory, etc.  The various forms and functions of these may be presented as under.

 

4.3.3.1 Indicative /m∂/

 

Indicative or declarative /m∂/ is marked only in negative constructions such as negative declarative or negative ye/no-question. Examples :

 

(1)  m -và-m∂-r

 

       NEG-GO-IND-FUT

‘(I) will not go’

(2)  m -và-m∂-r

 

      NEG-GO-IND-Q (YN)

‘Didn’t (you) go?’

(3) m -niη-m∂-n∂̀

 

      NEG-be-IND-COP

‘(It) is not’

 

4.3.3.2 Imperative /lù/

 

The imperative is marked by the suffix /lu/, as in:

 

(1)  và-lù

 

       go-imp

‘(You) go!’

(2)  thak-lù

 

      quick-IMP

‘Be quick’

 

 

4.3.3.3. WH-question /kh l /

 

Interrogative /kh l / is used with ‘wh-words’ (pronominals and adverbials) and ‘non-wh-words’ (verbs).  With verbs it occur in negative constructions.  Examples :

 

 

(1) n∂

khìpa-kh l

      you

who-Q (WH)

‘who are you?’

(2) n∂

và-k∂̀

m -và-kh l

      you

go-OR

NEG-go-Q(WH)

      Did you go or not?’ (Lit: You went or went not?)

(3)  à

Àton

m -niη-kh l

       3SG

A.

NEG-be-Q(WH)

‘Is she Aton or not?’

 

 

4.3.3.4. Yes/no-question /lá/

 

In a ‘yes/no’-question, /lá/ is suffixed to the verb or nominal, as in :

(1) à

Àton-lá

     3SG

A. -Q(YN)

‘Is she Aton?’

(2) à

Àton

m -niη-m∂-lá

     3SG

A.

NEG-be-IND-Q(YN)

‘Is she not Aton?’

(3) à

Àton-n∂̀

m -niη-m∂-lá

      3SG

A. –COP

NEG-be-IND-Q (YN)

      ‘she is Aton, isn’t it?’

 

4.3.3.5. Hortative /s∂̀/

 

/s∂̀/ is suffixed to express an exhortation, as in :

(1)   i-thùm

và-s∂̀

        1-PL

go-HOR

‘Let’s go’

(2)   thak-s∂̀

 

       quick-HOR

‘Let’s be quick’

 

4.3.3.6. Entreative /k∂̀/

 

/k∂̀/ is suffixed to verbs to express request of offering, etc., as in :

 

(1)  i  và-k∂̀

 

       I go-ENTR

‘Let me go’ (request /offer)

(2)  i-n∂̀

sá-mì-k∂̀

       I-NOM

do-give-ENTR

‘Let me do (it) (for you)’

 

4.3.3.7. Optative /rǘnò/

 

The optative suffix /rǘnò/ expresses realizable wishes or hopes, as in :

 

(1) v∂̀rè-n∂̀

so-nìo-rǘnò

      God-NOM

bless-give-OPT

‘May God bless you’

(2) ci

thà- rǘnò

      that

so-OPT

‘So be it’ (Lit: Let that be so)

4.3.3.8. Dubitative /m∂̀ráo/

 

The Dubitative suffix /m∂̀ráo/ expresses the meaning ‘perhaps it is so’, ‘it is likely to.....’ etc.  Thus, consider the following examples :

 

(1) Àton   

rá-m∂̀ráo-w∂̀

      A.

come-DUBI-NFUT

     ‘Aton is likely to come/Aton might come’

(2)  ci

thà-m∂̀ráo-l∂̀

      that

so-DUBI-Q

‘Can that be so?'

 

 

 

 

4.3.3.9. Potential/capability /r∂̀r/, /s p/, /thúk/

 

The verbs /r r/ ‘can’, ‘be able’, /s∂̀p/ ‘can’, ‘be fit’, ‘be enough’, and /thùk/ is preferably used to express ability;  / s∂̀p/ is often ambiguous as it may express ability of a doer in respect of an action, or the quantity/ quality of the object.  Thus, consider the following :

(1)   i 

hi

zà-s∂̀p-∂̀

        I

this

eat-POT-NFUT

       ‘I can eat this’ (because : (1) I have physical strength or (2) the rice is not too much)

(2)   i 

hi

zà-r∂̀r-∂̀

        I

this

eat-POT-NFUT

       ‘I can eat this’ (because : (1) I have physical courage or (2) I have physical strength)

(3)   i 

hi

khaη-thùk-∂̀

        I

this

eat-POT-NFUT

       ‘I can lift this’ (because I am strong)

 

 

4.3.3.10.    Permission/possibility /pay/

 

The verb /pay/ ‘be easy’ is used to express either permission or possibility or both.  examples are :

 

(1) n∂

và-pai-y∂̀

     you

go-POSB/PERM-NFUT

    ‘You can/may go’ (permission/possibility)

(2) n∂         i-li

sào-pai-r

      you       I-DAT

beat-PERM-FUT

     ‘You can beat me’ (permission)

 

4.3.3.11.    Probability /l∂̀-pai/

 

Probability may be expressed by infixing the adverbial particle / l∂̀/ ‘also’ between the modal verb /pai/ ‘be easy’ and the main verb or nominal stem, as in:

 

(1) à

Àton-l∂̀-pai-y∂̀

     3SG

a.-ALSO-PROB-NFUT

     ‘She might be Aton’ (Lit: Being Aton is also easy)

(1) n∂

yon-l∂̀-pai-y∂̀

     you

wrong-ALSO-PROB-NFUT

     ‘May be you are wrong’

 

l∂-pai-suffixed forms does not allow further suffixation of future tense even if an action is to take place in the future.  Thus, consider :

 

(3) Àton

rá-l∂̀-pai-y∂̀

     A.

come-ALSO-PROB-NFUT

     ‘May be Aton will come/Aton may come’

(4) Àton

rá-l∂̀-pai-r∂̀

     A.

come-ALSO-PROB-FUT

     ‘?Aton will may come’

 

 

4.3.3.12.    Desiderative /ηái/

 

The auxiliary verb /ηái/ ‘want’ is suffixed to verbal stems to express the sense of ‘wanting’ or ‘desire’. Examples :

 

(1) n∂

và-pai-y∂̀

      you

go-POSB/PERM-NFUT

     ‘You can / may go’ (permission/possibility)

(2) n∂

i-li

sào-pai-r∂̀

      you

I-DAT

beat-PERM-FUT

     ‘You can beat me’ (permission)

 

 

4.3.3.13.    Obligatory/necessitative /ph∂̀lùη/, /η∂̀yí/

 

/ph∂̀lùη/ ‘must’ expresses obligation or compulsion ; /η∂̀yí/ ‘ought to’ expresses obligation, or necessity, Thus, consider the following examples :

 

  (1)     i

và-ph∂̀luη-r∂̀

            I

go-OBLG-FUT

          ‘I must go’ (compulsive/ obligatory)

  (2)     n∂

và-η∂̀yí-y∂̀

            you

go-OBLG-NFUT

          ‘you  ought/need to go’ (necessary, reasonable, advisable)

 

 

 

4.3.3.14.    Impudency /rèη/ ‘dare’

This modal suffix expresses meanings such as ‘to be brave enough to do something difficult or dangerous’, or ‘to be rude or foolish enough to do something that one has no right to do’ Thus, consider the following :

 

(1)  I-máη

pí-rèη-∂̀

       I-ONLY

Sleep-DARE-NFUT

     ‘I dare (to) sleep alone’ (I’m not afraid)

(2)  I

m∂̀-haη-rèη-m∂-n∂̀

       I

NEG-say-DARE-IND-COP

     ‘I dare not speak’ (I’m afraid/ I feel shy)

 

4.3.3.15.     Advisability / suggestive /thu/

 

This suffix expresses advice or suggestion indicating the sense of ‘do this thing instead of wasting time in other things’.  Examples :

 

(1)  láirik-pa

pa-thu-lù

       book-SPEC

read-SUG-IMP

      ‘Study! (instead of wasting time watching TV, etc.)’

(2)  á-rúi

và-thu-r

       now

go-SUG-FUT

       ‘(I) will go now (instead of unnecessarily delaying)’

(3) v∂̀rè- wùi

wùη-r∂̀m

pha-thu-lù

      God-GEN

kingdom

seek-SUG-IMP

     ‘Seek (ye) (the) kingdom of God (instead of indulging in worldly pleasures)

 

4.3.3.16.    Prohibitive / šàr/

 

This suffix expresses the meaning ‘God/heaven forbids .....’, as in :

 

(1) 

sá-th∂̀t- šàr-∂̀

       man

Do-kill-FORBID-NFUT

      ‘(Thou) shalt not kill’ (Bible, Exodus 20 : 13)

(2)  à-vǘ

à-và-li

η∂̀mǘ- šàr-∂̀

       FX-mother

FX-father-DAT

Fight-FORBID-NFUT

       ‘One should not fight with (his/her) parents’

 

4.3.3.17.    Presumptive /m∂/

 

The presumptive marker /m∂/ expresses believing something to be true because it is very likely, as in :

 

(1) Àton

rá-m∂-r

      A.

come-PRESUM-FUT-SFE

     ‘Aton will come (as expected/usual/believed)’

      (though it is late now, etc.)

(2)  i

và-m∂-r

       I

Go-PRESUM-FUT

‘I will go (as expected/usual/believed)’

(though I don’t want to go, etc.)

 

4.3.3.18.    Subjunctive /ní/, /sí/, /kí/, /lǘ/

 

These are various subjunctive forms used in subordinate clauses or independent clauses to express hypothesis of no factuality /ní/, /sí/, /kí/, /lǘ/ are some of hte most commonly used contracted form of combining the subjunctive marker /i/ with modal auxiliaries.

 

(1) /ní/.  This is a contracted form of combining the copula /n∂̀/ and subjunctive /i/.  /ní/ is suffixed to nominals to express condition or concession.  Thus, compare (1) with (2) and (3) :

 

(1)  hi

sìna-n∂̀

       This

Gold-COP

‘This is gold’

(2) hi

sìna-ní

k∂̀-cì-y∂̀

phá-r

      this

Gold-COP+SBJNC

NOMZ-that-TOP

Good-FUT

      ‘If this were gold (it) will be good’

(3) I

n∂-ní

k∂̀-cì-y∂̀ ………

 

      I

you-COP+SBJNC

NOMZ-that-TOP

 

    ‘If I were you………….’

 

 

(ii) /sí/ is a contracted form of combining the hortative /s∂̀/ and subjunctive /i/.  I is used in clauses of hypothetical condition and is suffixable to any verbal stem/root, and the reference is to non-future or future time. Thus, examine the following:

(1) i-thùm

và-s∂̀

      IP-PL

Go-HOR

‘Let’s go’ (hortative/suggestive)

(2)  I-thùm

và-sí-lá

       IP-PL

Go-HOR+SBJNC-Q (YN)

     ‘Should/shall we go ?’ (IF you are willing?)

(3) và-sí

k∂̀-cì-y∂̀

phá-r

 Go-HOR+SBJNC

NOMZ-that-TOP

good-FUT

 

(iii) /kí/ is formed by combining entreative /k∂̀/ and subjunctive /i/.  It is suffixed to verbal stems/roots to express command, suggestion, or possibility, Thus, compare (1) with (2) and (3) below :

(1)  i

và-k∂̀

       I go-ENTR

‘Let me go’

(2)  n∂

và-kí-lá

k∂̀-cì-n∂̀

       you

go-ENTR+SBJNC-Q (YN)

 

(3)  n∂

và-kí

k∂̀-cì-n∂̀

       you

go-ENTR+SBJNC

 

‘You  are to go’

 

 

 

(iv) /lǘ/ is a contracted form of combining imperative /lù/ and subjunctive /i/.  It occurs in reported/quotative speech.  Thus, compare (1) with (2) below:

(1) n∂

rá-lù

      you

come-IMP

‘You come!’

(2) n∂-li

come-IMP+SBJNC

COMP-NFUT

     ‘You are ‘told’ to come’

 

4.4   Negation

 

Tangkhul-Naga has only one negative affix /m / which is used for both morphological and syntactic negation.  There are no other negators or negative auxiliaries except nasal  m or ∂  ‘no’ and ηk∂̀i ‘(I) won’t’.  The negative morpheme /m∂̀/ may be prefixed to infelcted or derived verbal stems, verbal nouns, adjectivals and adverbials.

 

There is also a negative indicative marker /m∂/ which is suffixed to inflected verbal, adjectival and adverbial bases.  The level-toned indicative /m∂/ has been often misinterpreted by many scholars as synonymous with the high-toned negative /m /.  The indicative /m∂/ occurs only in neative declaratives and negative ye-no questinons.  We can now examine some of the morpho-syntax of negation.

 

4.4.1. Negative /*/ or /*/ and / ηk∂̀i /

 

The nasal ** or ** ‘no’ and ηk∂̀i ‘no’, ‘I won’t’,  ‘Let me not’ are used in negative answersa or responses.  ηk∂̀i seems to be a contracted form of combining **  ‘no’ and entreative /k∂̀/ ‘let me . . .’  The occurrence of these negative words is illustrated in the following :

 

(1)  (a)  Yes-no question :

 

 

              n∂

à-li

théi-lá

              you

3SG-DAT

know-Q(YN)

‘Do you know him/her?

       (b) Negative  Response :

 

 

              ∂

(m -théi-m∂-n∂̀)

              no,

(NEG-know-IND-COP)

            ‘No! ((I) don’t know (him/her))

(2)  (a)  Wh-question :

 

 

              n∂

Àton-li

m∂- théi-kh l

              you

A.-DAT

NEG-know-Q (WH)

              ‘Do you know Aton or not?’

       (b) Negative response :

 

 

             ηk∂̀i

m∂- théi-m∂-n∂̀

 

              no,

NEG-know-IND-COP

‘No!((I) don’t know (her))’

(3)   (a) Imperative :

 

 

              i-li

mì-lù

 

            I-DAT

give-IMP

‘Give me’

      (b) Negative response :

 

 

             ηk∂̀i

(, m∂̀-mì-m∂-r )

 

             no

(NEG-give-IND-FUT)

‘No!((I) won’t give)’

 

4.4.2. Negation of NP, VP, ADJP and ADVP

 

As mentioned above, negative the morpheme / m∂/ is prefixed to inflected verbal, adjectivl, adverbial and verbal noun bases to negate them.  In case of noun phrases, the verb bases to negate them.  In case of noun phrases, the verb /níη/ ‘to be’ is negated.  Negation of the various phrases is illustrated below :

 

(1)  (a) à

Àton-n∂̀

 

            3SG

A. –AOP ‘She is Aton’

 

      (b) à

Àton

m -niη-m∂-n∂̀

            3SG

A. –

NEG-be-IND-COP

‘She is not Aton’

(2)  (a) Àton

k∂̀-phá

mì-n∂̀

            A.

NOMZ-good

person-COP

           ‘Aton is a good person’

      (b) Àton

m -k∂̀-phá

mì-n∂̀

            A.

NEG-NOMZ-good

person-COP

           ‘Aton is a bad person’

           (Lit : Aton is a person not being good)

(3)  (a) Àton

k∂̀-phá

m -niη-m∂-n

            A.

NOMZ-good

person

NEG-be-IND-COP

           ‘Aton is not a good person’

      (b) Àton

m -k∂̀-phá

m -níη-m∂-n∂̀

             A.

NEG-NOMZ-good

person

NEG-be-IND-COP

             ‘Aton is not a bad person’

             (Lit: Aton is not a not-good person)

(4)  (a) Àton-wùi

k∂̀- phá

théi-y∂̀

             A.-GEN

NOMZ-good

know-NFUT

            ‘(I) know Aton’s goodness’

       (b) Àton-wùi

m -k∂̀-phá

théi-y∂̀

             A.-GEN

NEG-NOMZ-good

know-NFUT

            ‘(I) know Aton’s badness’

             (Lit: Knew Aton’s not being good)

(5)  (a) Àton

rìη- phá-t∂

rìη-∂̀

             A.

live-good-ADV

live-NFUT

            ‘Aton’s lives happily’

       (b) Àton

m - rìη-phá-l

rìη-∂̀

             A.

NEG-live-good-CP

live-NFUT

            ‘Aton lives unhappily’

       (c) Àton

rìη-phá-t∂

m - rìη- m∂-n∂̀

             A.

live-good-ADV

NEG-live-IND-COP

            ‘Aton does not live happily’

 

Note that in negating adverbials the adverbializer /t∂/ is replaced by the particle /l∂̀/ ‘also’, ‘even’, as in (5) (b) above.  This is discussed in more detail in the following

 

4.4.3. Constituent negation and emphatic denial

 

Tangkhul-Naga has also a complex type of negation, whereby the negative marker attaches itself semantically to one of the non-verbal constituents of the clause, such as the subject, direct/indirect object, nominal/pronominal predicate or adverb.  In constituent negation not only does the event not occur with the listed participants, but one of the participants is not even involved.  As a speech-act, normal VP negation.  And the semantic effect of such negation on the noun phrase in question is to render  it non-referring.

 

In negation of this type /á-kh∂̀-l∂̀/ ‘one also’,  ‘even one’ occurs with nouns and /kh∂̀-l∂̀/ ‘one also’, ‘even one’ is  suffixed to pronominals and adverbs.  Thus, consider the following sentences :

 

(1)  (a) mì

á-kh∂̀

rá-i

            person

FX-one

come-NFUT

‘One person came’

      (b) mì

á-khà-l∂̀

m -rá-m∂-n∂̀

            person

FX-one-ALSO

NEG-come-IND-COP

           ‘No one came’(Lit: one person also did not come)

(2)  (a) khì-pa

rá-kh l

 

            what-SPEC

come-Q (WH)

 

            who

 

‘who came?’

      (b) khì-pa- kh∂̀-l∂̀

m -rá-m∂-n∂̀

 

          who-ONE-ALSO

NEG-come-IND-COP

          ‘Nobody/no one came’

            (Lit: Who-one-also did not come)

(3)  (a) khì-pa-li

mì-rǘ-kh l

 

            who-DAT

give-FUT+SBJNC-Q(WH)

 

            ‘Whom would (you) give?’

      (b) khì-pa-li-kh∂̀-l∂̀

m -mì-lù

 

          who-DAT-ONE-ALSO

NEG-give-IMP

          ‘Don’t give to anybody’

            (Lit: Don’t give to who-one-also)

(4)  (a) k -cì-li

và-r -kh l

 

            WH-that-LOC

go-FUT-Q (WH)

‘Where will (you) go?’

      (b) khì-cì-li-kh∂̀-l∂̀

m -và-m∂-r

 

          WH-that-LOC-ONE-ALSO

NEG-go-IND-FUT

          ‘(I) will not go anywhere’

            (Lit: Will not go where one also)

 

4.4.4. Double negation

Double negation occurs quite legitimately in two-clauses or single-clause sentences, where, in a sense, the two negatives do indeed cancel each other out.  Often, double negative constructions form ‘litotes’  -- the figure of speech in which, for the sake of effect, affirmation is expressed as negative or its contrary.  The occurrence of double negative in the language is illustrated Below :

 

(1)  n∂

m -kh∂̀-và

m -sá-pai-m∂-r

       you

NEG-NOMZ-go

NEG-do-POSB-IND-FUT

      ‘You may not do not going’ (You must go)

(2) (a) Àton

m -k∂̀-zìk

m -niη-m∂-n∂̀

           A.

NEG-NIMZ-black

NEG-be-IND-COP

           Àton is not unblack’ (= very/rather black)

Compare  (b) below :

    (b) (i) Àton

m -zìk-m∂-n∂̀

 

               A.

NEG-black-IND-COP

‘Aton is not black’

         (ii) Àton

k∂̀-zìk

m -niη-m∂-n∂̀

              ‘It is not that Aton is black’

 

 

4.4.5. Negative intensifiers

 

There are two intensifiers /phút/ ‘at all’ and /lak/ ‘very much’, ‘definitely’.  /phút/   occurs only in negative constructions, and /lak/ occurs both in negative and non-negative constructions.  In negative sentences /lák/ expresses the meaning ‘at all’ or ‘ever’.  Examples :

 

(1)  (a)   i

và-lak-r

 

              I

go-INTSF-FUT

‘I will definitely go’

      (b)   i

m -và-lák-m∂-r

 

              I

NEG-go-INTSF-IND-FUT

‘I will never go’

      (c)   i

m -và-phút-m∂-r

 

             I

NEG-go-INTSF-IND-FUT

‘I won’t go at all’

(2)  (a)   i

và-lùi-r

 

              I

go-AGAIN-FUT

‘I’ll go again’

      (b)   i

m -và-lùi-m∂-r

 

              I

NEG-go-AGAIN-IND-FUT

‘I ‘ll not go again’

      (c)   i

m -và-lùi- phút-m∂-r

 

             I

NEG-go-AGAIN-INTSF-IND-FUT

‘I’ll never not go again at all’

      (d)   i

m -và-lùi-lák-m∂-r

 

             I

NEG-go-AGAIN-INTSF-IND-FUT

‘I’ll never go again’/ ‘I’ll definitely not go again’

 

 

4.4.6. Negative raising

Negation of a subordinate clause may optionally be expressed by negating the verb of the higher clause, that is, the verb belonging to the meaning category of subjective opinion, such as / thà/ ‘seem’, ‘appear’, ‘be so’, Examples :

 

(1) à

k∂̀-k∂̀zà

m -thà-m∂-n∂̀

     3SG

NOMZ-sick

NEG-seem-IND-COP

     ‘He/she doesn’t seem to be sick’

compare (2) below :

 (1) à

m -k∂̀-k∂̀zà

thà-ì̀

     3SG

NEG-NOMZ-sick

seem-NFUT

     ‘He/she seems to be not being sick’

 

4.5. Complementizers and subordinating conjunctions

 

In many languages of the world (including many of the Tibeto-Burman languages), a that complementizer is marked by the verb ‘say’.  The Tangkhul-Naga situation is very complex as both the verb ci ’be that’and the conjunctive particle /t∂/ can function as complementizers and quotatives.  Structurally, the verb ci behaves like any other verb and occurs with all the inflectional morphemes, including the conjunctive particle /t∂/.  The morpheme /t∂/is also rather remarkable.  It occurs with a wide range of functions, including a that-complementizer; and adverbial subordinator introducing clauses of purpose, result, reason; and a component of miscellaneous adverbials.  Detailed discussion of the complementizers is beyond the scope of this paper, and we will examine only basic morpho-syntactic characteristics of them.

 

4.5.1. Noun versus predicate complementation

 

The most challenging semantic and syntactic problems presented by the phenomena of Tangkhul-Naga sentential complementation are found within the different types of complementation.  Of particular difficulty is the question of accounting for the distribution of the particle /t∂/ and the verb cì ‘be that’.  One major difference between them is that /t∂/ is realized as the predicate ‘complementizer’ while ci is realized as the ‘Noun complementizer’. To illustrate this syntactic test, we can observe the effects of applying the role of cleft formation to sentences with t∂ COMP and ci COMP.

 

(1) Noun complementation :

(a) Àton

thì-hái-r∂

k∂̀-cì

páo

ci

sà-ì

      A. 

die-PP-PERF

Nomz-that

news

that

hear-NFUT

     ‘(I) heard the news that Aton has died’

(b) i-n∂̀

k∂̀-sà-∂̀

Àton thì-hái-r∂

k∂̀-cì

páo

ci-n∂̀

      I-NOM

NOMZ-hear-TOP

A. die-PP-PERF

COMP

news

that-COP

‘What I heard was the news that Aton has died’

(2) Predicate complementation:

(a) ) i-n∂̀

Àton-thì-hái-r∂ t∂

sà-ì

 

 

 

       I-NOM

A.  die-PP-PERF CP

hear-NFUT

 

 

 

      ‘I heard (that) Aton has died’

*(b) i-n∂̀

k∂̀-sà-v∂̀

Àton-thì-hái-r∂ n∂̀

I-NOM

NOMZ-hear-TOP

A.  die-PP-PERF  CP-COP

‘? What I heard was (that) Aton has died’

 

In the above examples, the cleft sentence of (1) (a) is grammatical while that of (2) (b) is not. This is because the underlined sequence of (1) (a) is an NP because it can be ‘isolated’ by the cleft construction of (1) (b), while the underlined sequence of (2) (a) is not an NP because its occurrence in the pre-copular (NP) position of (2) (b) is precluded.  Similar conclusions can be reached by observing the effects of applying such transformations as the topicalization.

 

4.5.2. cì and t∂ as  quotatives

 

Both the verb ci and the adverbial particle t∂ can introduce direct speech where the exact words of the speaker are quoted.  Examples :

 

(1)  (a) à-li

cì-lù

 

            3SG-DAT

dog

that-IMP

‘Call him/her ‘Dog’’

      (b) à-li

t∂

ho-lù

 

            3SG-DAT

dog

QT

call-IMP

‘Call him/her ‘Dog’’

(2)  (a) i

và-r

cì-y∂̀

 

 

            I

go-FUT

that-NFUT

‘(Somebody) said : ‘I’ ll go”

      (b) i

và-r

t∂

haη-∂̀

 

 

            I

go-FUT

QT

say-NFUT

‘(Somebody) said : ‘I’ ll go”

 

 

4.5.3. cì as a full verb

 

cì behaves as a ‘full’ verb by taking all the inflectional affixes.  In actual usage cì can be synonymous with a number of verbs such as ‘say’, ‘think’, ‘act’, etc., that is, cì can be represented by a number of verbs.  t∂ occurs as a complementizer, quotative particle or subordinator occurring with cì or verbs synonymous with ci.  Thus, consider the following examples :

 

(1) nào

ci

c∂̀p-r

cì-t∂

sá-i

      baby

DET

cry-FUT

that-CP

do-NFUT

‘The baby looked/acted/behaved to start crying’

(2) (a) nào

ci

m -zà-m∂-r

cì-háo-w∂̀

 

            baby

DET

NEG-eat-IND-FUT

that-PUNC-NFUT

 

‘The acted baby that he/she won’t eat’

      (b) nào   

ci

m -zà-m∂-r -t∂

c∂̀p-háo-w∂̀

            baby

DET

NEG-eat-IND-FUT-CP

cry-PUNC-NFUT

(3) (a) nào

ci

c p-kí

cì-hái-r∂

          baby

DET

cry-SBJNC

that-PP-PERF

          ‘The baby is about to cry’

           (Lit : It has beeb that the baby is to cry)

      (b) nào

ci

c p-kí-t∂

mai

mèr-phok-hái-r∂

            baby

DET

cry-SBJNC-CP

face

mellow-start-PP-PERF

           ‘The baby was started ‘mellowing’ his/her face in order to cry’

(4) (a)  n∂

phá-i

m -cì-lù

 

             you

good-NFUT

NEG-that-IMP

 

            ‘Don’t ‘think’ that you are good’

      (b)  n∂

phá-i-t∂

m -ph∂̀niη-lù

 

             you

good-NFUT-CP

NEG-think-IMP

 

            ‘Don’t think that you are good’

 

 

 

4.5.4. cì and t∂ in frozen combinations

 

cì and t∂ occur in a number of more-or-less frozen combinations in which their earlier complementizer identity may or may not be seen. They are combined with other morphemes to form idiomatic expressions and subordinating conjunctions.  (For the latter, see discussion on ‘Adverbials’ in chapter 3).  Some of the resulting combinations are exemplified below :

 

(1)  cì-lak-∂̀

 

       that-INTSF-NFUT

‘That’s it!’

(2) ci

thà-rǘnò

 

      that

like-opt

 

     ‘So be it’ (Lit : Let (it) be like that)

(3)  ci

thà-t∂

sá-t∂

       that

like-ADV

do-ADV

       ‘Thus; (Lit : Doing like that)

 (4) k - thà-t∂

 

       WH-like-ADV

‘How?’

 (5) k -cì-wùi-vaη

 

       WH-that-GEN-BEN

‘why?’ (Lit: for which)

(4) m - cì - k

 

       NEG-that-ENT-SFE

‘Let (me) not’ (please don’t mind)

 

Thus, we have seen that ci and t∂ have a comples morpho-syntax and semantic relations.  People tend to say or act what they think, and to believe what they say or perform; the pragmatic distance is small between saying/acting and thinking, feeling, believing, and knowing. The verb cì corresponds to what is ‘thought’, ‘said’, ‘performed’ or ‘believed’.  For instance, when the thought is not actually uttered, the verb cì introduces the clause which expresses the thought.  When there is no actual speaking, the sense of the verb ci is metaphorical rather than literal; the thought or idea is left unsaid.  In other words, cì ‘be that’ is what is ‘say’ complementizer is many other languages, and that it has a syntactically conditioned counterpart, the adverbializer or conjunctive particle t∂, which occurs with verbs ‘substituting’ cì.

 

4.6 Non-finite verb forms

 

Non –finite verbs in Tangkhul-Naga are formed by affixing the nominalizer or non-finite marker /k∂̀/ ~ /kh∂̀/ and a number of adverbial particles.  (See also the discussion on adverbial particles in chapter 3).  Non-finite verbs have both positive and negative forms.  Furthermore, they can be inflected for aspect, mood and modal auxiliaries, except tense.

 

The following are some examples of infinitivalized, participialized or nominalized verbal forms in the language.  In this section /k∂̀/ ~ /kh∂̀/ will be interpreted variably as either nominalizer (NOMZ) or non-finite marker (NF).  Affixation of /k∂̀/ ~ /kh∂̀/ is of two types  - - (1) prefixation and (2) infixation; it is prefixed to single-root verbs and infixed by ‘prefixing’ to the final root in compound and agglutinated verbs.

 

4.6.1. Non-finite non-future

 

Non-future infinitives, present participles or verbal nouns are formed by affixing /k∂̀/ ~ /kh∂̀/ to the verb roots.  Examples :

 

Finite : (1)  pí-y∂̀

 

 

      I sleep-NFUT

‘I sleep/slept

 

(2) i n∂-li

y∂̀η- šì- y∂̀

 

      I you-DAT

look-bad-NFUT be hostile

 

     ‘I’ m hostile to you’

Non-finite :

 

 

(1) thak-t∂

k∂̀-pí

t∂̀m-lù

     quick-ADV

NF-sleep

learn-IMP

      ‘Learn sleeping/to sleep early’

(2) y∂̀η-k∂̀- šì

m -phá-m∂-n∂̀

 

      look-NF-bad

NEG-good-IND-COP

 

     ‘Hostility/to be hostile is not good

 

 

4.6.2. Non finite past participle

 

Non-finite past participle is formed by infixing/k∂̀/ ~ /kh∂̀/ before the past participle marker hái.  (hái is a grammaticalized verb meaning ‘to keep/leave’).  Examples :

Finite : h∂m

ci

kai-hái-r∂

 

             pot

that

break-PP-PERF

‘The pot has broken’

Non-finite:

 

 

 

(1) kai-k∂̀-hái

h∂m

ci

khui-rá-lù

     break-NF-PP

pot

that

take-come-IMP

    ‘Bring the broken pot’

(2) h∂m

ci

kai- k∂̀-hái

thà-ì

      pot

that

break-NF-PP

seem-NFUT

     ‘The pot seems to have been broken’

 

 

 

4.6.3. Non-finite progressive participle

 

The progressive participle consists of the non-finite marker followed by the delexicalized verb sà ‘to hear’.  Thus, in the following example sa behaves as a progressive (PROG) marker:

 

Finite :

 

 

 

          i

z∂̀t-sà-ì

 

 

         I

go-hear-NFUT

 

 

‘I was going’ (Lit :? (Somebody) heard that I go)

 

         I

z∂̀t-k∂̀-sà

théi-y∂̀

 

         I

go-NF-PROG

see-NFUT

‘(Somebody) saw me going’

 

 

 

 

 

4.6.4. Non-finite perfect/past continuous participle

 

Perfect or past continuous participle is formed by combining past participle hai ‘leave’ and progressive ša ‘hear’ and infixing the non-finite marker.  (Possible translations are marked with ‘?’). Examples :

Finite : n∂

z∂̀t-hái-r∂-sà-ì

 

             you

go-PP-PERF-PROG-NFUT

 

‘? You had gone/You had been going’

Non-finite : n∂

z∂̀t-hái-r∂-k∂-sà

théi-y∂̀

                    you

go-PP-PERF-NF-PROG

know-NFUT

                   ‘(I) knew you having gone/having been going’

 

4.6.5. Nominalized verb and verb agreement

 

Non-finite or nominalized forms of Tangkhul-Naga verbs come in a variety of ‘semi-finite’ forms, that is, forms that retain some aspect marking.  However, there is no agreement in tense.  Thus, a distinction is made between finite verb predicates and non-finite/nominalized verb predicates.  Nominalized forms can take only the nominal or negated verbal predicative copula /n∂̀/ which finite verb forms do not take.  This is exemplified below :

 

1. Verbal predicate

      i

cao

rèη-∂̀

      I

deer

hunt-NFUT

‘I hunt deer’

2. Nominal predicate

 (1) hi

cao-n∂̀/*∂̀

 

       this

deer-COP/*NFUT

‘This is (a) deer’

(2)   i

cao

kh∂̀-rèη-n∂̀/*∂̀

        I

deer

NOMZ-hunt-COP/*NFUT

       ‘I’ m deer-hunting/I hunt deer’

(3)   i

cao

kh∂̀-rèη-∂̀-n∂̀

        I

deer

NOMZ-hunt-AGT-COP

‘I’ m (a) deer-hunter’

3. Negated verbal predicate :

(1) hi

cao

m -níη-m∂-n∂̀

      this

deer

NEG-be-IND-COP

‘This is not (a) deer’

(2)  i

cao

m -rèη-m∂-n∂̀

       I

deer

NEG-hunt-IND-COP

‘This is not (a) deer’

(3)  i

cao

m -kh∂̀-rèη-m∂-n∂̀

       I

deer

NEG-NOMZ-hunt-COP

(4)  i

cao

kh∂̀-rèη

m -rèη-m∂-n∂̀

 

 

4.7 Serial verbs

 

The term ‘serial verb’ is generally used to refer to a surface string of verbs or verb-like items which occur within what appears to be a single clause.  In such constructions there is typically a semantic relationship of some sort between the two verbs (hereafter referred to as V1 and V2 ).  Thus, consider the following examples :

 (1)  i-n∂̀

khui-rá-r

 

       I-NOM

take-come-FUT

‘I will bring’

(2) i-n∂̀

šào-mì-r

 

      I-NOM

beat-give-FUT

 

    ‘I will beat (someone for somebody’)

(3) ph∂̀niη-úη-r

 

 

     think-come-FUT

‘(I) will remember’

 

 

 

In (1) above ra ‘come’ function as a directional complement to khui ‘take’ providing the gloss ‘bring’.  In (2) above mì ‘give’ is lexically empty and ambiguous in meaning between ‘dative’ and ‘benefactive’; thus the sentence may mean either ‘to beat (to teach a lesson); ‘to beat (for someone’s benefaction) or ‘to beat’ (on behalf of someone). mì ‘give’ in such constructions, thus, occurs as an ‘explicator’ (Explicator compound verb are discussed separately in the next section).  In (3) above, the verbs  seem to work as a compound or an idiom, since the meanings in isolation (ph∂̀niη ‘think’ and uη ‘come’ or ‘return to earlier place’) are not directly relatable to their gloss ‘remember’ in the serial sentence.

 

Few linguist have attempted to define ‘serial verbs’ with any degree of rigor, and not only has the notion of ‘serial verbs’ sometimes been vague, but a satisfactory definition of the category ‘verb’ has not been always available.  Thus , in case of Tangkhul-Naga, in particular, ‘verb; has been the subject of considerable confusion.

 

To make any progress at all in the present study, the indecision or debate as to whether the phenomena in question are correctly to be analyzed as phrase structure (syntactic) or lexical phenomena, or by means of transformations, is not explored.

 

4.7.1. Identifying characteristics

 

The principal criteria which a ‘serial verb’, having the sequence of the form V1V2, must meet for inclusion in this section are :

 

(i)                  Both V1 and V2 have only one overtly expressed (syntactic) subject;

 

(ii)                No conjunctive particle or tense-aspect marker should separate (by infixation) the verb in sequence;           

 

(iii)               Both V1 and V2 should be interpreted as having the same tense;

 

(iv)              Negation (marked by prefixation to V1 ) and tense-aspect-modality (marked by suffixation to V2) apply to the whole string;

 

(v)                Both V1 and V2 must be lexical verbs, and must be capable of appearing as the only verb in a simple sentence, excepting the delexicalized verbs like šuη ‘reach’ and transitivized/causativized verbs such as th∂̀t ‘kill’, or khai ‘break’, which occurs only in V2 position.

 

 

We have spoken of ‘serial’ sequences as if they consisted of two verbs exactly, but in fact this is often not the case, up to seven verbs (including modal auxiliaries) may occur in a sequence (probably more in theory), as in :

 

m∂̀-v∂̀-η∂̀phìt-khai- khui-úη-mì-ηái-m∂-n∂̀

NEG-go-hit-break-take-come-give-want-IND-COP

‘(I) don’t want to go, hit, break and bring (for some-one)’ (Lit : Don’t want to go-hit-break-take-come-give)

 

such constructions, as in the above sentence, would require more strenuous lexical, semantic and syntactic analysis, and in the present study it will considerably simplify our exposition if we stick with the fiction that just two verbs are involved.

 

4.7.2. Serialization as coordination and subordination

 

Serial verbs may be coordinating or subordinating.  In coordinate structure two or more verbs expressing different  successive actions or states simultaneous with another state or action (but having the same subject) are merely joined together without separating the sequence by any grammatical morpheme.  In this case two or more sentences are thrown or contracted into one, and the verbs are coordinate in meaning.  Thus, consider the following :

(1)  Àton-n∂̀

rá-khui-r

       A. –NOM

come-take-FUT

‘Aton will come and take’

(<Aton will come.  Aton will take).

 

 

In subordinate sense, V1 is the principal or adverbial and V2 is either the principal or an auxiliary verb, supplying, as it were, directionality, forming or introducing a complement or adjunct; or supplemental, forming part of the verb phrase.  The actions expressed by both V1 and V2 are simultaneous and in an ‘internal’ or inseparable relation or connection. In this case, the supplemental verb V2 is coordinate only in form but subordinate in sense.  Thus, consider (2) below :

 

(2)  Àton-n∂̀

khui-rá- r

       A. –NOM

take-come- FUT  ‘Aton will bring’

 

(1) and (2) above both consist of the verbs khui ‘take’ and ra ‘come’ but mere ordering of the verb results coordinating the subordinating structures.  This shows that there is a strict ordering relationship between the verbs, and changes in the ordering results in quite different meanings or actions.

In the following discussion, subordinating and coordinating structures will be demonstrated by changing the ordering relationship as far as practicable.

 

4.7.2.1. Verbs occurring as V1 or V2

 

We can now examine some of the verbs which can occur as V2 in coordinating and subordinating constructions.  Oftens, the supplemental V2 expresses various meanings.  Verbs can be classified into different groups according to their semantic and syntactic relationships.

 

1.      Directional and motion verb ra ‘come’ may express varied but related meanings in V1 and  V2 positions, Examples :

 

(a) Àton-n∂̀

rá-ho-ì

 

      A. –NOM

come-call-NFUT

‘Aton came and call’

(b) Àton-n∂̀

ho-rá-ì

 

      A. –NOM

call-come-NFUT

 

‘Aton came calling (one person after another)/ and bring (him/her)/ Aton came calling/ has been calling (somebody some name)’

 

2.      The verb y η ‘look’ expresses either ‘to look’, ‘to see’ or ‘to try’, ‘to see what (it is) like’, as in :

(a) cycle

ci

i-n∂̀

tòη-y∂̀η-k∂̀

     cycle

that

I-NOM

ride-see-ENT

‘Let me ride the cycle and see (whether I can ride or whether is comfortable or not, etc.)

(b) i

y∂̀η-và-r∂̀

 

      I

look-go-FUT

 

‘I will look away/over (that side) /I will go looking (all the way)’

 

3.      Directional verbs occur both as V1 and V2 as in :

(a) i-n∂̀

n∂m-rá/và/sok/z∂η/ká/tá-r∂̀

     I-NOM

push-come/go/out/in/up/down-FUT

‘I will push towards/away/out/in/up/down’

(b) i-n∂̀

rá/và/sok/z∂η/ká/tá-n∂m-r∂̀

     I-NOM

come/go/go out/go in/go up/go down and push’

 

 

4.      Verb of motion or change of position/state thui ‘be up/away’ occurs V1 or V2, and expresses the meaning ‘rise’, ‘rise up’, ‘be away’, etc.  Thus compare (1) with (2), (3), (4) and (5)  below :

(1) thui-ù-s∂̀

 

arise/away-IMM-ENTR

‘Let’s get up/go away’

(2)      i

thui-m∂̀thùk-∂̀

           I

awake-revive-NFUT

‘I woke up (form sleep)’

(3)      i

η∂̀k∂r-thui-y∂̀

           I

spring up-rise   up-NFUT

         ‘I curled up/get up (from lying position)’

(4)      i

thui-p∂m-∂̀

           I                      

get up-sit-NFUT

         ‘I got up (form curled-up position) and sat’

(5)     i

thui- η∂̀níη-∂̀

          I

get  up-stand-NFUT

         ‘I got up (form sitting position) and stood’

 

5.      The verb roro ‘fall (on the surface)’, as V1, occurs with only one verb ta ‘be down’ and as V2, occurs with many verbs.  Thus, consider the following examples

(1)  pàη

m∂̀cìk-r

t

rór-tá-lù

       hand

wash-FUT

water

let  fall-down-IMP

     ‘(You) pour down water, I’ll wash (my) hand’

(2) pàη-li

t

hèi-rór-lù

 

     hand-LOC

water

pour-let

fall-IMP

    ‘Pour water on the hand’

(3) ph∂̀hón-li

à- ši

c∂̀k-rór-∂̀

 

     shirt-LOC

blood

jerk-let

fall-NFUT

‘Blood jerked and fell on the shirt’

 

6.      Perception verb théi ‘know’, ‘see’ occurs as V1 with directional verb, and as V2 it occurs with many verbs.  In V2 position it means either ‘see’ or ‘know how to’. Examples :

 

(1) n∂

àyár-li

k∂̀-p∂m

théi- šok-∂̀

      you

outside-LOC

NOMZ-sit

see out-NFUT

‘(I) saw you sitting outside’

(Lit: Saw out you sitting outside)

(2)   i

šok-théi-y∂̀

       I

out-know-NFUT

‘I went out and saw/I know how to go out’

 

4.7.2.2. Fixed V2 verbs

 

There are a number of verbs which cannot occur in isolation or V1 position. They are either auxiliaries, delexicalized or causativized verbs.  We will call them ‘fixed V2’.  The following are some of them.

 

1.      Causativized verbs like th∂t ‘kill’, khai ‘break’ cannot occur either in V1 position or in isolation. To convey single verbal idea, these verbs always take the ‘support’ or ‘dummy’ verb sa ‘do’.  Examples :

 

(1)  i-li

sá-th∂̀t-lù

                            thi= ‘die’

       I-DAT

do-die+CAU-IMP

‘ (You) kill me’

(2)  i-n∂̀

sá-khai-r

                    [kai = ‘break’]

      I-NOM

do-break+CAUS-FUT

‘I will break’

(3) Àton-li

kap-th∂̀t-r

 

      A.-DAT

shoot-kill+CAUS-FUT

 

‘(I) will shoot dead Aton’ (Lit: Will shoot kill Aton)

 (4) h∂m

ci

y∂̀p-khai-y∂̀

       pot

that

hit-break+CAUS-NFUT

‘(Somebody) hit-broke the pot’

 

2.      ‘Arrival’ verb šuη ‘reach’ occurs with a number of motion and perception verbs.  Examples :

(1) à-thùm

Delhi

á-kh∂̀m∂

rá- šuη-r

      3-PL

D.

FX-tomorrow

come-reach-FUT

‘They will arrive Delhi tomorrow’

(Lit: They will come-reach Delhi tomorrow)

(2) k -l∂

mat-th∂̀η

và- šuη-t∂

p∂m-r

      and

Sunday

go-reach-ADV

stay-FUT

‘And (they) will stay (in Delhi) till Sunday’

(Lit:? They will go on staying and reach Sunday

 

3.      The verb vái ‘to wear’, ‘to utilize’ occurs in a number of ‘purposive’ constructions. Examples :

 

n∂

con- ší

ci

khì

sá-vái-r -kh l

you

cloth-rag

that

what

do-PURP-FUT-Q

‘What will you do with the rag?’

(Lit : What will you do to utilize the rag?/ What will you use the rag for?)

(b) phéi

mìt-vái-r

      leg

wipe-PURP-FUT

‘(I) will use (it) for wiping feet’

(c) thào-vái-rú-nò

tárá-kh∂̀

šai-lù

     fat-PURP-OPT

many

eat-IMP

‘In order to be /become fat, eat a lot’

(d) thào-kh∂̀-vái

tárá-kh∂̀

šai-lù

     fat-NF-PURP

many

eat-IMP

    ‘Eat a lot (so as) to become fat’

 

 

4.      The verb η∂̀nào occurs only in V2 position, and carries the meaning ‘play’, ‘do far fun’, ‘do jokingly/without seriousness’. Examples :

(a) z∂̀t-η∂̀naò-s∂̀

 

 

     walk-PLAY-ENTR

‘Let’s walk just to while away’

(b) m∂̀-šì-lù

thà-t∂

haη-η∂̀nào

k∂̀-cì-n∂̀

      NEG-mind-IMP

like-ADV

say-PLAY

NOMZ-that-COP

‘(You) don’t mind, it is that (I am ) just saying jokingly’ (without any seriousness or intention to hurt you)

(c) i-ní

η∂̀mǘ-η∂̀nào-w∂̀

     1-DU

fight-PLAY-NFUT

    ‘We two fought just for fun’

 

In conclusion, many interesting problems remain in connection with serial verbs.  Several important questions remain to be answered.  For example, how does serialization relate to coordination and subordination ? Again, what is the relationship between productive syntactic serial constructions and ‘fully lexicalized’ ones? To answer these and other problems, we will have to look deeper into a mixture of syntactic, semantic and lexical facts.

 

4.8.  Explicator compound verbs

Like ‘idiomatic’ compound verbs and serial verbs, explicator compound verbs (henceforth ECV) are formed by combining two (or more) verbs - - V1+V2(+V3. . .).  However, unlike in the former structures, V2 in ECV is only an explicating or modifying element of V1.  To quote Abbi : “The main meaning of the verb is contained in V1 while V2 is lexically emptied, i.e. grammaticalized.  V2 in most cases functions as a modifying or explicating element of V113.  That is, V2S lose their verbal semantic content and have acquired grammatical function.  ECVs are thus the result of grammaticalization of verbs.  Lexical verbs typically name events, actions, processes, or states.  But, over times, certain verbs have undergone historical reanalysis as pre/post-positions, complementizers, adverbials, auxiliaries, and tense-aspect markers.

 

According to Abbi (1994) explicators in South Asian languages indicate three types of meanings - - ‘aspectual’, ‘adverbial’ and ‘attitudinal’ - -  each of which can further be divided into several subtypes. In Tangkhul-Naga there are about ten verbs which may function as explicators in a number of constructions They may indicate aspectual, adverbial or attitudinal meanings, such as perfective, progressive, continuative, dative, benefactive, additional, intensity, request, humility, and so on.

 

In the following discussion I have not attempted to distinguish the various ‘minute’ meaning, as which one should apply is not always apparent from translations in the source language.  In some cases, we face difficulty in

 

 


13A. Abbi (1994). Semantic Universals in Indian Languages. Shimla: Indian Institute of Advanced Study. p. 44.

 

Identifying the meaning, or assigning the status, or explicators in structures containing two or more delexicalized verbs, that is V2, V3 ........ Sometimes it is not clear (or debatable) as to whether V1 can also function as an explicator.  This will tested.  The verbs that can occur as explicators are discussed in order.

 

4.8.1. khui ‘take’ constructions

The verb khui ‘take’ may occur as V2 denoting self-benefaction, direction, etc.  Examples:

[cì in isolation is mid]

(1) fü

cì-li

ho-khui-lù

 

      dog

that-DAT

call-take-IMP

 

‘Call the dog (to your side/direction)/

‘Call the dog (and not let it go (away)’ (Control)

(2) mì-pìη-n∂̀

n∂-li

ho-khui-r

      man-PL-NOM

you-DAT

dog

call-take-FUT

      ‘People will call you ‘Dog’ (if they repeatedly keep on calling you ‘dog’)’ (Habit)

(3) khai-éin∂

k∂̀t∂̀t-khui-lù

      knife-INSTR

cut-take-IMP

     ‘Cut (it) with (the) knife’ (Instrumental)

(4) m∂̀lùη

kh∂̀-vat-wùi-va

i-n∂̀

k∂̀phe-khui-y∂̀

      heart

NOMZ-burst-GEN-BEN

I-NOM

slap-take-NFUT

     ‘As I was angry, I slapped (somebody)’ (for my satisfaction)

(5) rìη- k∂̀phe-khui-lù̀

NOMZ-burst-GEN-BEN

I-NOM

slap-take-NFUT

     live-good-take-IMP

 

 

 

     happy

‘Enjoy (yourself)’

 

 

 

khui ‘take’, as V1, also seems to function as an explicator in certain constructions, such as:

 

 

(6) n∂

k∂̀l∂̀t-t∂-wùi

tùi

khui- šai-lù

      you

reverse-ADV-GEN

word

take-eat-IMP

‘Eat your own word (you deserve what you get)’ (Scolding)

Compare :

 

 

 

(7) šai- khui-lù̀

      eat-take-IMP

‘Eat

(for you own benefit)’ (Self-benefaction)

 

 

4.8.2. mì ‘give’ constructions

 

Give constructions are also widespread in the language.  There is a tendency for mì ‘give;, as an explicator, indicating either ‘dative’ or ‘benefactive’.  Thus, consider :

 

(1) i-n∂̀

Àton-li

šào-mì-y∂̀

     I-NOM

A.-DAT

beat-give-NFUT

‘I beat Aton’ (to teach her a lesson)

(? Dative/Other-benefaction)

(2) i-n∂̀

Àton-wùi-vaη

šao-mì-y∂̀

      I-NOM

A.-GEN-BEN

beat-give-NFUT

‘I beat (somebody) for / on behalf of Aton’ (Other-benefaction)

(3) yarùi-n∂̀

cíη-ri-éin∂

p∂m-mì-lù

      public-NOM

peace-MODF-ASS

sit-give-IMP

‘All (of you) sit quietly!’ (Request)

 

4.8.3. và ‘go’ constructions

 

The verb và ‘go’, as an explicator, denotes such meanings as progressive/inceptive –progressive or ‘direction away’. Unlike in many other languages, va is not a completive or perfective aspect marker in the language.  It cannot occur in constructions in which the action, process or state is instaneous. Thus, (1) below is not acceptable whereas the rest are well-formed :

 

(1) à-k∂̀hùη

k∂̀t∂̀t-∂̀kh∂̀

η∂̀làη-t∂

thì-và-háo-r

      FX-neck

cut-COND

instant-ADV

die-go-PUNC-FUT

    ‘If (one’s) neck is cut(he/she) will die instantly’

(2) mì-thít-rì

šai-l∂̀k∂

thì-và-ηái-y∂̀

      man-die-medicine

eat-CP

die-go-want-NFUT

     ‘(I) want to consume poison ad die gradually/slowly’

(3) Àton

thào-và-hái-r∂

 

      A.

fat-go-pp-PERF

 

    ‘Aton has become fat/fatter and fatter’

     (She was not fat) (Inceptive-progressive)

(4) cì-vak

y∂̀η-và-lù

 

      that-DIR

look-go-IMP

‘Look that side’

 

 

(Direction away)

 

 

4.8.4. rá ‘come’ constructions

 

The verb rá ‘come’, as an explicator, denotes continuative or immediacy and ‘direction towards’, as in :

 

(1) nào

ci

k∂̀-sáη-kh∂̀

c∂p-rá-hái-r∂̀

     child

that

NOMZ-long-UNIT

cry-come-PP-PERF

(2) Àton

i-wùi

šim-li

rá-rá-lì

      A.

I-GEN

house-LOC

come-come-PPROG

     ‘Aton is coming to/towards my house’

      (Approaching/Immediacy + Direction)

(3) i-li

y∂̀η-rá-lú

 

      a.DAT

look-come-IMP

 

     ‘Look at/towards me’ (Direction towards)

(4) Àton

thi-rá-lì

 

      A.

die-come-PPROG

 

    ‘Aton is about to die’ (Immediacy)

 

 

 

4.8.5. thui ‘be away’ constructions

 

The verb thui ‘be away’ expresses meanings such as ‘to be away’, ‘to change action, process or state’ and ‘attitude’.  Thus, compare thui as V1 in (1) and as V2 in the rest :

 

(1) Àton

thui-hái-r∂

 

      A.

away-leave-PERF

‘Aton has left/gone away’

(2) Àton

z∂̀t-thui-hái-r∂

 

      A.

go-away-PP-PREF

‘Aton has gone away’

(3) Àton

thào-thui-hái-r∂

 

      A.

fat-away-PP-PERF

(Change of state)

     ‘Aton has become fat’ (She was not fat earlier)

 

 

4.8.6. uη ‘return’ constructions

The verb uη ‘to return to earlier place or state’, as V2 expresses the meanings ‘again’, ‘direction towards’, etc.  Thus, compare (1) (in which uη occurs as V1) and the rest below:

 

(1) i

á-rúi

uη-r

      I

FX-now

return-FUT

‘I will go back now’ (to the place/home/camp or any place from where I had come)

(2) Àton

thào-úη-hái- r∂

 

    A.

fat-return-PP-PERF

(Recurrence)

   ‘Aton has become fat’ (She used to be fat and she had become thin once)

(3) y∂̀η-úη-lù

 

 

      look-return-IMP

(Direction backwards)

 

 

 

 

 

4.8.7. s∂η ‘put’ constructions

 

s∂η ‘put’, as V2, indicates the meanings ‘addition’, ‘inclusion’ or ‘involvement’. Examples :

 

(1)  Àšàη

Àton-li

thì-s∂η-∂̀

      A.

A.-DAT

die-put-NFUT

     ‘Àšàη ‘loves’ Àton very much’

      (Lit : Àšàη ‘dies’ on Àton)

(2) Àšàη

rài-li

thi-s∂η-∂̀

       A.

war-LOC

die-put-NFUT

(involvement)

     ‘Àšaη died in (the) war’ (He was involved in the war)

(3) zat

khui-s∂η-lù

 

      rice

take-put-IMP

‘Take more rice’  (Extra)

(4) Àšàη-li-l∂̀

i-si

loη-li

ho-s∂η-r

      A-DAT-ALSO

I-CL

group-LOC

call-put-FUT  (inclusion)

    ‘(We) will call in Àšàη also in our group/gang’

 

 

4.8.8. šok ‘be out’ constructions

 

šok’ to go out’, ‘to move out’, as an explicator, denotes directional, adverbial and attitudinal meanings such as ‘do and get over with’, ‘suddenness’, etc. Examples:

 

(1) ot

ci

kùp- šok- hái-r∂

      work

that

finish-out-PP-PERF

‘(I) have finished the work’ (and now the work doesn’t bother me anymore)

(2)  nào

ci

àvǘ-li

théi-t∂

c∂̀p- šok-háo-w∂̀

       child

that

mother-DAT

see-CP

cry-out-PUNC-NFUT

      ‘On seeing his/her mother the child cried’

(3) Àton-li

váo- šok-lù

 

 

      A.-DAT

shout-out-IMP

(Direction)

 

     ‘Shout for  Aton’ (who is sitting outside)

 

 

4.8.9. šán ‘count/enumerate’ constructions

 

The verb šan ‘to count’, ‘to enumerate’, as V2, denotes meanings such as ‘prolongation’ ‘fixedness’, intensity’ or ‘addition’.  Thus, consider the following constructions :

 

(1) Àton-wùi

pàη

siη- šán-∂̀

      A.-GEN

hand

hold-count-NFUT  (Prolongation)

    ‘(Somebody) held Aton’s hand unreleasingly’ (thus not letting her go)

(2) Àton-li

y∂̀η- šán-∂̀

 

      A.-DAT

look-count-NFUT

(Fixedness)

     ‘(Somebody) looked at Aton fixedly’

 (3) i-n∂̀-l∂̀

Àton-li

šào-šán-r∂̀

       I-NOM-ALSO

A.-DAT

beat-count-FUT    (Additional)

     ‘I also will beat Aton’ (on top of being beaten by others)

(4) i-li

Àton-n∂̀

k∂̀th∂̀k-šán-∂̀

      I-DAT

A.-NOM

kick-count-NFUT (Intensity/Iterative)

     ‘Aton kicked me forcefully/repeatedly’

 

 

4.8.10. tá ‘fall/be down’ constructions

 

The verb tá ‘to fall’, ‘be down’, as an explicator, expresses meanings such as ‘carelessness’, ‘without hesitation/interruption’, etc., as in :

 

(1) i-n∂̀

Àton-li

kh∂̀r r-tá-i

      I-NOM

A.-DAT

scold-fall-NFUT

‘I scolded Aton’ (without hesitation or without care for her reaction)

(2) Àton-li

y∂̀p-tá-r

 

      A-DAT

hit-down-FUT

 

      ‘(I) will hit Aton’ (mercilessly)

(3) Àton- n∂̀

phéisa

k∂̀-toη-∂̀

šìcìn-tá-hái-r∂

     A-NOM

money

NOMZ-full-ATTRI

spend-down-pp-PERF

    ‘Aton has spend all the money’ (carelessly in a spendrift way)

(4) n∂-wùi

k∂̀-ph∂̀niη- niη

haη-tá-lù

 

      you-GEN

NOMZ-think-REDU

say-down-IMP

 

‘Say whatever is in you mind’ (without hesitation or pause

(Lit: Tell down your thinking thinking)

 

 

4.8.11. p∂m ‘sit’ constructions

 

The verb p∂m ‘sit’, as in explicator, indicate continuation of an action.  Examples :

 

(1) cì-li

m -η∂̀niη-p∂m-lù

 

      that-LOC

NEG-stand-sit-IMP

 

     ‘Don’t keep on standing there’

(2) Àton

c∂̀p-p∂m-háo-w∂̀

 

       A.

cry-sit-PUNC-NFUT

 

     ‘Aton went/kept on crying’

To sum up.  we have seen that there are a handful of verbs which may lose, in V2 position, their verb status and become ‘modifying’ or ‘explicating; elements.  Many questions remain to be answered.  For instance, what verbs (or what remain to be answered.  For instance, what verbs (or what types of verbs) can participate in explicator compound verbs, and what determines the order of verbs in ‘explicator compound verbs’ constructions? On a broader front, we still do not know why explicator compound verbs occur in some languages but not in others.  To answer these questions, we need to take into pragmatic, typological and areal/cross-linguistic studies.